Skip to main content
Log in

Response to Joint Attention in Toddlers at Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Prospective Study

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Response to joint attention (RJA) is impaired in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and is pivotal to social and communication development. Response to joint attention was examined at 14 and 24 months in 51 children at high risk for autism (siblings of children with autism). Outcome groups at age 3 years included ASD (n = 16), broader autism phenotype (n = 8), and non-broader autism phenotype (n = 27). The ASD group made minimal improvement in RJA between 14 and 24 months, but stability of RJA across tasks increased for all three groups. Significantly, lower RJA was observed for the ASD group at 24 months. Response to joint attention performance at 14 months predicted ASD outcome. Response to joint attention is an important screening and early intervention target.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychological Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study. Psychological Medicine, 25, 63–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Infant Behavior & Development, 55, 467–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. (1995). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: Bradford. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development, and simulation of everyday mindreading. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G., & Grover, L. (1988). The origins of referential communication in human infancy. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 5–24). New York: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G., & Grover, L. (1990). Joint visual attention, manual pointing, and preverbal communication in human infancy. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance 13: Motor Representation and control (pp. 605–624). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 6(4, Serial No. 176).

  • Charman, T., Taylor, E., Drew, A., Cockerill, H., Brown, J., & Baird, G. (2005). Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 500–513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corkum, V., & Moore, V. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In C. Moore, & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 61–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J., McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay. Child Development, 73, 345–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deak G. O., Flom, R. A., & Pick, A. D. (2000). Effects of gesture and target on 12- and olds’ joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. Developmental Psychology, 36, 511–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DiLavore, P. C., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (1995). Pre-linguistic autism diagnostic observation schedule. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 355–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, S. E., & Rutter, M. (1977). Infantile autism: A genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 297–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, S. E., Santangelo, S. L., Gilman, S. E., Piven, J., Landa, R. Lainhart, J., et al. (1999). Predictors of cognitive test patterns in autism families. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 1117–1128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E., Bolton, P., Prior, J., Jorda, H., & Rutter, M. (1997). A family study of autism: Cognitive patterns and levels in parents and siblings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 667–683.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, W. A., Jarvis, K. L., Osann, K., Laulhere, T. M., Straub, C., Thomas, E., et al. (2005). Brief report: Early social communication behaviors in the younger siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 657–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven: Yale University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Freeman, S. F. N., & Paparella, T. (2006). Joint attention and symbolic play in young children with autism: A randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 611–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Lang, J., Cicchetti, D. V., & Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Interrater reliability of clinical diagnosis and DSM-IV criteria for autistic disorder: Results of the DSM-IV autism field trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 163–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R. (2005, October). Jumpstarting social and communication development in autism through early intervention. Presented at The MIND Institute, University of California, Davis.

  • Landa, R., Holman, K., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (in press). Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry.

  • Landa, R., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2006). Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 629–638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R., Folstein, S., & Isaacs, C. (1991). Spontaneous narrative discourse performance of parents of autistic individuals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1339–1345.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M. M., Gayle, J. O., Chase, G. A., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Social language use in parents of autistic individuals. Psychological Medicine, 22, 245–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Le Couteur, A., Bailey, A., Goode, S., Pickles, A., Robertson, S., Gottesman, I., et al. (1996). A broader phenotype of autism: The clinical spectrum in twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 785–801.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., & Ramsden, C. A. (2006). Dyadic orienting and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 185–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lempers, J. D. (1979). Young children’s production and comprehension of nonverbal deictic behaviors. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 135, 93–102.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and imagination: The origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised—A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1989). Stability of assessment results of autistic and non-autistic language-impaired children from preschool years to early school age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 575–590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luyster, R., Richler, J., Risi, S., Hsu, W. L., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., et al. (2005). Early regression in social communication in autistic spectrum disorders: A CPEA study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 311–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., Angelopoulos, M., & Bennett, P. (1997). The role of movement in the development of joint visual attention. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 83–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morissette, P., Ricard, M., & Decarie, T. G. (1995). Joint visual attention and pointing infancy: A longitudinal study of comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 163–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning: AGS edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Neal, R. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. International Review of Mental Retardation, 23, 139–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining social deficits in autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657–659.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorders versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Shalev, R. S., & Gross-Tsur, V. (2003). Language abilities of siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 914–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, D. L., Fein, D., Barton, M. L., & Green, J. A. (2001). The modified checklist for autism in toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 131–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64 (1, Serial No. 256).

  • Stone, W. L., Lee E. B., Ashford, L., Brissie, J., Hepburn, S. L., Coonrod, E. E., et al. (1999). Can autism be diagnosed accurately in children under 3 years?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 219–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yirimiya, N., Gamliel, I., Pilowsky, T., Feldman, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Sigman, M. (2006). The development of siblings of children with autism at 4 and 14 months: Social engagement, communication, and cognition. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 47, 511–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, E., & Dawson, G. (2005). Validation of the phenomenon of autistic regression using home videotapes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 889–895.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 157–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (2002). Communication and symbolic behavior scales-developmental profile. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing, (First Normed Edition).

    Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention training for children with autism using behavior modification procedures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 456–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1992). Preschool Language Scale-3. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool Language Scale-4. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors express their gratitude for the funding provided by grants MH59630 and 154MH066417 (Studies to Advance Autism Research and Treatment) from the National Institute of Mental Health, funding from NIMH 5T32MH20033-07 (postdoctoral fellowship for Michelle Sullivan), and funding from Pathfinders for Autism, National Alliance for Autism Research, Cure Autism Now, and Coalition for Autism, awarded to Rebecca Landa (PI). We thank the families for their generous participation in and commitment to this research. Sincere appreciation is also expressed to staff, without whose efforts this research could not be executed: Sarah Beal, Cornelia Taylor, Jim Mancini, Allison Nelson, Amy Knecht, Julie Rusyniak, Seton Lindsay, Dana Christina, Laura Becker, Julie Cleary, Katherine Holman, Katie Owens, Amy Reese, Allison O’Neill, Amy Falk, Audrey Thurm, Melissa Martin, Kelley Shaw, Sharon Loza, Lesley Strunge, Kathryn Gleeson, Andrea Schanbacher, Andrea Golloher, Kirsten Basore, Kate Brooks, Monae Johnson, Sue Miller, Erica Gee, Juhi Pandey, and Rachel Weiman.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca Landa.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sullivan, M., Finelli, J., Marvin, A. et al. Response to Joint Attention in Toddlers at Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Prospective Study. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 37–48 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0335-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0335-3

Keywords

Navigation