Skip to main content
Log in

Beyond Pragmatics: Morphosyntactic Development in Autism

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Language acquisition research in autism has traditionally focused on high-level pragmatic deficits. Few studies have examined grammatical abilities in autism, with mixed findings. The present study addresses this gap in the literature by providing a detailed investigation of syntactic and higher-level discourse abilities in verbal children with autism, age 5 years. Findings indicate clear language difficulties that go beyond what would be expected based on developmental level; specifically, syntactic delays, impairments in discourse management and increased production of non-meaningful words (jargon). The present study indicates a highly specific pattern of language impairments, and importantly, syntactic delays, in a group of children with autism carefully matched on lexical level and non-verbal mental age with children with developmental delays and typical development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. When analyses were repeated, excluding the child who had been talking for only 4 months, results were identical.

  2. The MLU routine alone calculates only the number of words per utterance, rather than the number of morphemes, which is the variable of interest. Thus, after running MLU on each transcript to find the number of utterances spoken by the child, FREQ was used to find the complete frequency listing, assessed word by word to determine whether each word consisted of one or more morphemes. Additional morphemes were then incorporated into the measurement of utterance length.

  3. For participants who produced fewer than 100 utterances, subscale scores were not computed (because of potential differences across groups on subscales). Thus, the subscale analyses are likely more conservative since they were based on a smaller group of participants.

  4. In this scheme, some utterances are likely to be erroneously labeled as jargon, when the experimenter fails to recognize a target utterance even though it is a known word. Utterances from children with poor articulation will be over-included, making comparisons between the autism and DD group more conservative, as the latter are likely to have misarticulations.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/ 4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

  • Adams, R., & Khoo, S. (1993). Quest, the interactive test analysis system. Melbourne: ACER.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., & D’Angiola, N. (1997). Cohesion in the discourse interaction of autistic, specifically language-impaired, and normal children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(1), 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental language disorder: I. The children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolucci, G. (1982). Formal aspects of language in childhood autism. In Advances in child behavior analysis and therapy (Vol. 2, pp. 159–18). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

  • Bartolucci, G., & Albers, R. J. (1974). Deictic categories in the language of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 19, 131–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S. J., & Streiner, D. (1980). Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10(1), 39–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1997). Origins of language disorders: A comparative approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 447–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Lahey, J., Hood, L., Lifter, K., & Fiess, K. (1980). Complex sentences: Acquisition of syntactic connectives and the semantic relations they encode. Journal of Child Language, 7, 235–261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Rocissano, L., & Hood, L. (1976). Adult–child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 521–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). The frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad”: Narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(2), 193–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpentieri, S. C., & Morgan, S. B. (1994). Brief report: A comparison of patterns of cognitive functioning of autistic and nonautistic retarded children on the Stanford-Binet – Fourth Edition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 215–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. In. The Hague: Mouton.

  • Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 12(3), 349–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, A., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Drew, A., Klein, K., Baird, G., et al. (1999). Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(5), 719–732.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curcio, F., & Paccia, J. (1987). Conversations with autistic children: contingent relationships between features of adult input and children’s response adequacy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(1), 81–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms in the first two years of life: A preliminary population study of infantile autism. European Archives of Psychiatric and Neurological Science, 283, 169–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalgleish, B. (1975). Cognitive processing and linguistic reference in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 5(4), 353–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 267–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demuth, K. (1996). Collecting spontaneous production data. In D. McDaniel, C. McKee, & H. Smith Cairns (Eds.), Methods for assessing children’s syntax (pp. 3–22). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Diehl, J. D., Bennetto, L., & Young, E. C. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 87–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test-3. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eigsti, I. M., & Bennetto, L. (2001). Syntactic and memory functions in young children with autism. San Diego, CA: Paper presented at the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology.

  • Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1979). Autism is not a disorder of language. Boston, MA: Paper presented at the New England Child Language Association.

  • Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism: A review. Psychological Medicine, 29(4), 769–786.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foster (1986). Learning discourse topic management in the preschool years. Journal of Child Language, 13, 231–250.

  • Fowler, A. E. (1980). A comparison of normal and retardate language equated on MLU. Boston, MA: Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Child Language Development.

  • Godber, T., Anderson, V., & Bell, R. (2000). The measurement and diagnostic utility of intrasubtest scatter in pediatric neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56(1), 101–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 3, pp. 41–58). NY: Academic Press.

  • Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1975). The recall of digits by normal deaf and autistic children. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 203–209.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62(4), 782–796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Department of Sociology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (1984a). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in autistic children: A critique and replication of the findings of Bartolucci, Pierce, and Streiner, 1980. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 127–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (1984b). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in autistic children: a critique and replication of the findings of Bartolucci, Pierce, and Streiner, 1980. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(2), 127–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism and Development Disorders, 33(1), 3–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Russell, J. (1997). Language profiles in children with autism: Theoretical and methodological implications. Autism, 1, 57–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Morris, R., & Delis, D. (1991). WAIS as a neuropsychological instrument (Manual). New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, E., Paul, J., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 87–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 287–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, T. T., & Evans, L. D. (1996). Stanford-Binet: Fourth edition short forms with underachieving and learning disabled students. Psychological Reports, 79(1), 47–50.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 195–225). New York: Wiley.

  • Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (1996). Language level and nonverbal social-communicative behaviors in autistic and language-delayed children. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(11), 1542–1550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & DiLavore, P. C. (1998). Autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES Database. Dublin, OH: Discovery Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T., & Xu, F. (1992). Over-regularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(4), 1–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L. (2004). Matching strategies in cognitive research with individuals with high-functioning autism: Current practices, instrument biases, and recommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 19–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. (1996). An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain and Language, 52(3), 411–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M. R., & Hall, L. C. (1979). Comprehension of transitive and intransitive phrases by autistic, retarded, and normal children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 12, 103–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prizant, B., & Duchan, J. (1981). The functions of immediate echolalia in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorder, 46, 241–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I. (1991). Autistic children: Diagnosis and clinical features. Pediatrics, 87(5), 751–760.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I., & Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain and Development, 25(3), 166–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubino, R. B., & Pine, J. M. (1998). Subject-verb agreement in Brazilian Portuguese: What low error rates hide. Journal of Child Language, 25(1), 35–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rumsey, J. M., Duara, R., Grady, C., Rapoport, J. L., Margolin, R. A., Rapoport, S. I., et al. (1985). Brain metabolism in autism: Resting cerebral glucose utilization rates as measured with positron emission tomography. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42(5), 448–455.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2, 435–450.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Mawhood, L., & Howlin, P. (1992). Language delay and social development. In P. Fletcher & D. Hall (Eds.), Specific speech and language disorders in children: Correlates, characteristics, and outcomes (pp. 63–78). London: Whurr.

  • Sattler, J. M. (1992). Assessment of children: Revised and updated third edition. San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. (1990). Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H., Rescorla, L., Tager-Flusberg, H., Fowler, A., & Sudhalter, V. (1991). Relation of utterance length to grammatical complexity in normal or language-disordered groups. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 23–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & MacDonald, M. C. (1999). A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 569–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A. M., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Speech and Language Hearing Research, 44(5), 1097–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W., & Yoder, P. (2001). Predicting spoken language level in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 5(4), 341–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Boyle, M., Streiner, D., & Duku, E. (2003). Predictors of outcome among high functioning children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 520–528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The development of contingent discourse ability in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1123–1134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Calkins, S. (1990). Does imitation facilitate acquisition of grammar? Evidence from the study of autistic, Down’s syndrome and normal children. Journal of Child Language, 17, 591–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M., & Chadwick-Dias, A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale: Fourth Edition. Chicago, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volden, J., & Lord, C. (1991). Neologisms and idiosyncratic language in autistic speakers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 109–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wanska, S. K., & Bedrosian, J. L. (1986). Topic and communicative intent in mother–child discourse. Journal of Child Language, 13(3), 523–535.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

This research was supported by the NIMH (R03 grant # MH61032-01), a Young Investigator award from the Journal of Language Learning, and a Dissertation Award from the Society for Science of Clinical Psychology, to IME. We are especially grateful to the parents and children that participated in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Inge-Marie Eigsti.

Appendix Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn) Items

Appendix Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn) Items

Table 7  

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Eigsti, IM., Bennetto, L. & Dadlani, M.B. Beyond Pragmatics: Morphosyntactic Development in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 1007–1023 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0239-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0239-2

Keywords

Navigation