Skip to main content

Overlapping and Distinct Cognitive Impairments in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity and Autism Spectrum Disorder without Intellectual Disability

Abstract

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are commonly comorbid, share genetic liability, and often exhibit overlapping cognitive impairments. Clarification of shared and distinct cognitive effects while considering comorbid symptoms across disorders has been lacking. In the current study, children ages 7–15 years assigned to three diagnostic groups:ADHD (n = 509), ASD (n = 97), and controls (n = 301) completed measures spanning the cognitive domains of attention/arousal, working memory, set-shifting, inhibition, and response variability. Specific processes contributing to response variability were examined using a drift diffusion model, which separately quantified drift rate (i.e., efficiency of information processing), boundary separation (i.e., speed-accuracy trade-offs), and non-decision time. Children with ADHD and ASD were impaired on attention/arousal, processing speed, working memory, and response inhibition, but did not differ from controls on measures of delayed reward discounting, set-shifting, or interference control. Overall, impairments in the ASD group were not attributable to ADHD symptoms using either continuous symptom measures or latent categorical grouping approaches. Similarly, impairments in the ADHD group were not attributable to ASD symptoms. When specific RT parameters were considered, children with ADHD and ASD shared impairments in drift rate. However, children with ASD were uniquely characterized by a wider boundary separation. Findings suggest a combination of overlapping and unique patterns of cognitive impairment for children with ASD as compared to those with ADHD, particularly when the processes underlying reaction time measures are considered separately.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Notes

  1. AIC = 2000.6, Sample Size Adjusted BIC = 1994.0, Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test for 1 versus 2 classesp < .001; Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test for 2 versus 3 classesp = .199

  2. AIC = 7794.79, Sample Size Adjusted BIC = 7856.28, Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test for 1 versus 2 classesp = .047; Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test for 2 versus 3 classesp = .521

References

  • Baddeley, A. (1996). The fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A, 93(24), 13468–13472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the royal statistical society. Series B (Methodological), 289–300.

  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The Children’s Communication Checklist, Second Edition (CCC-2). London: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conners, C. K. (2008). The Conners 3rd Edition (Conners 3). North Tonawanda: Multi-Health System.

    Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2012). The social responsiveness scale, Second Edition (SRS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, B. A., Constantine, L. J., Hendren, R., Rocke, D., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 166(2), 210–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, N. O., & Kollins, S. H. (2012). Treatment for co-occurring attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 518–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, C. R., Wood, S. J., Anderson, V., Buchanan, J. A., Proffitt, T. M., Mahony, K., & Pantelis, C. (2003). Normative data from the CANTAB. I: development of executive function over the lifespan. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(2), 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.2.242.13639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). San Antonio: The Pscyhological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demurie, E., Roeyers, H., Baeyens, D., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2012). Temporal discounting of monetary rewards in children and adolescents with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science, 15(6), 791–800. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01178.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(3), 331–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Martino, A., Zuo, X.-N., Kelly, C., Grzadzinski, R., Mennes, M., Schvarcz, A., & Milham, M. P. (2013). Shared and distinct intrinsic functional network centrality in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 74(8), 623–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DuPaul, G., Power, T., Anastopoulos, A., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD Rating Scale—IV: Checklists, Norms, and Clinical Interpretation. NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fair, D. A., Bathula, D., Nikolas, M. A., & Nigg, J. T. (2012). Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with ADHD. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 109(17), 6769–6774. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1115365109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gargaro, B. A., Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Tonge, B. J., & Sheppard, D. M. (2011). Autism and ADHD: how far have we come in the comorbidity debate? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(5), 1081–1088.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 836–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 25–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J. G. (2009). Research electronic data capture (REDCap)--a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 42(2), 377–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang-Pollock, C. L., Karalunas, S. L., Tam, H., & Moore, A. N. (2012). Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: a meta-analysis of CPT performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 360–371. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027205.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hulst, B. M., Zeeuw, P., Bos, D. J., Rijks, Y., Neggers, S. F., & Durston, S. (2017). Children with ADHD symptoms show decreased activity in ventral striatum during the anticipation of reward, irrespective of ADHD diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(2), 206–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., & Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748–751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M.J., Conway, A., Hambrick, D.Z., & Engle, R.W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control Variation in Working Memory (pp. 21–48).

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Karalunas, S. L., Geurts, H. M., Konrad, K., Bender, S., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Annual research review: Reaction time variability in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders: measurement and mechanisms of a proposed trans-diagnostic phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(6), 685–710. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karalunas, S. L., Gustafsson, H. C., Dieckmann, N., Tipsord, J., Mitchell, S. H., & Nigg, J. T. (2017). Heterogeneity in development of aspects of working memory predicts longitudinal ADHD symptom change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(6), 774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karalunas, S. L., & Huang-Pollock, C. L. (2013). Integrating impairments in reaction time and executive function using a diffusion model framework. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(5), 837–850. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9715-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban, S. A., Friedman, L. M., & Kolomeyer, E. G. (2013). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(6), 795–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users' guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2001). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, J. H. (2014). Handbook of Biological Statistics (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Sparky House Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M., Loya, F., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2013). Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD: developmental trajectories and associations with symptom change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(9), 1005–1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miodovnik, A., Harstad, E., Sideridis, G., & Huntington, N. (2015). Timing of the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics, 136(4), e830–e837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S. H. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 455–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S. H., Wilson, V. B., & Karalunas, S. L. (2015). Comparing hyperbolic, delay-amount sensitivity and present-bias models of delay discounting. Behavioural Processes, 114, 52–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Musser, E. D., Hawkey, E., Kachan-Liu, S. S., Lees, P., Roullet, J. B., Goddard, K., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Shared familial transmission of autism spectrum and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(7), 819–827. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal Heterogeneity in Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder: Do We Need Neuropsychologically Impaired Subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1224–1230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (1999). The ADHD response-inhibition deficit as measured by the stop task: Replication with DSM-IV combined type, extension, and qualification. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27(5), 393–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T., Hinshaw, S. P., & Halperin, J. M. (1996). Continuous performance test in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Methylphenidate dose response and relations with observed behaviors. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25(3), 330–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nyden, A., Niklasson, L., Stahlberg, O., Anckarsater, H., Wentz, E., Rastam, M., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Adults with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD neuropsychological aspects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6), 1659–1668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.04.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 171–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Special Issue: Annual research review, 37(1), 51–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pirrone, A., Dickinson, A., Gomez, R., Stafford, T., & Milne, E. (2017). Understanding perceptual judgment in autism spectrum disorder using the drift diffusion model. Neuropsychology, 31(2), 173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puig-Antich, J., & Ryan, N. (1986). Kiddie schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia. Pittsburgh: Western Psychiatric Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (2002). A diffusion model account of response time and accuracy in a brightness discrimination task: fitting real data and failing to fit fake but plausible data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 278–291. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (2006). Modeling response signal and response time data. Cognitive Psychology, 53(3), 195–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.10.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., & Rouder, J. N. (2016). Modeling response times for two-choice decisions. Psychological Science, 9(5), 347–356. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raymaekers, R., Antrop, I., van der Meere, J. J., Wiersema, J. R., & Roeyers, H. (2007). HFA and ADHD: a direct comparison on state regulation and response inhibition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(4), 418–427. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390600737990.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rommelse, N., Geurts, H., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J., & Hartman, C. (2011). A review on cognitive and brain endophenotypes that may be common in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and facilitate the search for pleiotropic genes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1363–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rommelse, N. N., Franke, B., Geurts, H. M., Hartman, C. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010). Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(3), 281–295. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-010-0092-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronald, A., Larsson, H., Anckarsäter, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2014). Symptoms of autism and ADHD: A Swedish twin study examining their overlap. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronald, A., Simonoff, E., Kuntsi, J., Asherson, P., & Plomin, R. (2008). Evidence for overlapping genetic influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 535–542. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01857.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagvolden, T., Aase, H., Zeiner, P., & Berger, D. (1998). Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behavioural and brain research, 94(1), 61–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00170-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, J., Johnson, K. A., Garavan, H., Gill, M., & Gallagher, L. (2008). A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research in autistic spectrum disorders: Attention, inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanislow, C. A., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K. J., Kozak, M. J., Garvey, M. A., Heinssen, R. K., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2010). Developing constructs for psychopathology research: research domain criteria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 631–639. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020909.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Butcher, B. (2010). Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominately inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 1017–1027. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0951-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinzig, J., Bruning, N., Morsch, D., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Attention profiles in autistic children with and without comorbid hyperactivity and attention problems. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 20(4), 207–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114(1), 104–132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Meer, J. M., Oerlemans, A. M., van Steijn, D. J., Lappenschaar, M. G., de Sonneville, L. M., Buitelaar, J. K., & Rommelse, N. N. (2012). Are autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder different manifestations of one overarching disorder? Cognitive and symptom evidence from a clinical and population-based sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(11), 1160–1172 e1163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Mourik, R., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The Stroop revisited: a meta-analysis of interference control in AD/HD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(2), 150–165. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00345.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, A., & Voss, J. (2007). Fast-dm: a free program for efficient diffusion model analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 767–775. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192967.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336–1346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, V. B., Mitchell, S. H., Musser, E. D., Schmitt, C. F., & Nigg, J. T. (2011). Delay discounting of reward in ADHD: application in young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 256–264. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02347.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yerys, B. E., Kenworthy, L., Jankowski, K. F., Strang, J., & Wallace, G. L. (2013). Separate components of emotional go/no-go performance relate to autism versus attention symptoms in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 27(5), 537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by Simons Foundation #177894 and by National Institutes of Health R01 MH096773 (Fair), R00MH091238 (Fair), R01 MH086654 (Nigg), R37 MH059105 13 (Nigg), and K23 MH108656 (Karalunas). We thank Corinne Stevens, Libby Nousen, and Andrew Bontemps for assistance with data collection and preparation. We gratefully acknowledge the participants and their caregivers, as well as the graduate students, staff, and research assistants whose remarkable dedication has made this research possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sarah L. Karalunas.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest or financial disclosures to report.

Ethical Approval

All procedures were approved by the Oregon Health & Science University’s Institutional Review Board.

Informed Consent

Parents provided written informed consent and children provided written assent.

Electronic Supplementary Material

ESM 1

(DOCX 40 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Karalunas, S.L., Hawkey, E., Gustafsson, H. et al. Overlapping and Distinct Cognitive Impairments in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity and Autism Spectrum Disorder without Intellectual Disability. J Abnorm Child Psychol 46, 1705–1716 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0394-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0394-2

Keywords

  • Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
  • Autism spectrum disorders (ASD)
  • Executive function
  • Neuropsychology
  • Reaction time variability
  • Diffusion model