Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 34, Issue 1, pp 83–98 | Cite as

Story Recall and Narrative Coherence of High-Functioning Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Joshua J. Diehl
  • Loisa Bennetto
  • Edna Carter Young
Original Article

Previous research has found few quantitative differences between children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) and well-matched controls in the length, complexity, and structure of their narratives. Researchers have noted, however, that narratives of children with ASDs have an unusual and idiosyncratic nature. This study provides an analysis of narratives in 17 children with high-functioning ASDs and 17 typically developing children matched on age, gender, language abilities, and cognitive abilities. We examined story recall and narrative coherence. The study revealed no group differences in story length or syntactic complexity. Children with ASDs also did not differ from controls in their use of the gist of a story to aid recall, or in their sensitivity to the importance of story events. Children with ASDs did, however, produce narratives that were significantly less coherent than the narratives of controls. Children with ASDs appeared less likely to use the gist of the story to organize their narratives coherently. These findings are discussed with regard to their relationship to other cognitive and linguistic difficulties of children with ASDs.

autism spectrum disorders language narratives coherence pragmatics 

REFERENCES

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Applebee, A. N. (1978). The child's concept of a story. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bamberg, M. (1987). Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral, and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125.Google Scholar
  5. Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder: I. The children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boucher, J. (1981). Immediate free recall in early childhood autism: Another point of behavioural similarity with the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 72, 211–215.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1998). Fuzzy-trace theory and children's false memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 81–129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 267–291). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). ‘‘The frog at the bug and made his mouth sad’’: Narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 193–204.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Crais, E. R., & Lorch, N. (1994). Oral narratives in school-age children. Topics in Language Disorders, 14, 13–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms in the first two years of life: A preliminary population study of infantile autism. European Archives of Psychiatric and Neurological Science, 283, 169–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Davis, G. A., O’Neil-Pirozzi, T. M., & Coon, M. (1997). Referential cohesion and logical coherence of narration after right hemisphere stroke. Brain and Language, 56, 183–210.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Donahue, M. (1985). Communicative style in learning disabled children: Some implications for classroom discourse. In D. N. Ripich & F. M. Spinelli (Eds.), School discourse problems (pp. 97–124). San Diego, CA: College-Hill.Google Scholar
  15. Flory, K. S., Milich, R., Lemberger, C., Whirley, K., Hayden, A., & Strange, C. (2003). Why do people with ADHD have deficits in story production and comprehension? Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
  16. Fox, B. J., & Wright, M. (1997). Connecting school and home literacy experiences through cross-age reading. Reading Teacher, 15, 535–555.Google Scholar
  17. Frith, U. (1989). Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  18. Gopnik, A., Capps, L., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2000). Early theories of mind: What the theory can tell us about autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 50–72). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Gummersall, D. M., & Strong, C. J. (1999). Assessment of complex sentence production in a narrative context. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 52–64.Google Scholar
  20. Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1975). The recall of digits by normal deaf and autistic children. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 203–209.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Houston, G. (1997). The power of story: What I have learned as a writer and a teacher. Reading Teacher, 50, 382–395.Google Scholar
  22. Hymes, D. (1971). On communicative competence. In J. Pride & J. Holmes (Eds.), Sociolinguistics (pp. 269–293). Baltimore: Penguin.Google Scholar
  23. Johnson, R. E. (1970). Recall of prose as a function of the structural importance of the linguistic units. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 12–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Joliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine, 30, 1169–1187.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottleib, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new ‘advanced’ test of theory of mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 517–528.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 61–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kintsch, W. (1987). Psychological processes in discourse production. In H. Dechert & M. Raupach (Eds.), Psycholinguistic models of production. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  28. Landa, R. (2000). Social language use in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. In A. Klin, F. Volkmar, & S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 125–155). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  29. Landa, R., Martin, M., Minshew, N., & Goldstein, G. (1995). Discourse and abstract language ability in non-retarded individuals with autism. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.Google Scholar
  30. Lorch, E. P., Sanches, R. P., van den Broek, P., Milich, R., & Welsh, R. (1999). The relation of story structure properties to recall of television stories in young children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and nonreferred peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 293–309.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. Cohen & F. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 195–225). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  32. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leventhal, B., DiLavore, P., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the deficits of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–224.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lord, C., & Venter, A. (1992). Outcome and follow-up studies of high-functioning autistic individuals. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with autism (pp. 187–199). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  35. Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 239–251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Loveland, K., McEvoy, R., Tunali, B., & Kelley, M. L. (1990). Narrative story telling in autism and Down's syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 9–23.Google Scholar
  37. Loveland, K., & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative language in autism and the theory of mind hypothesis: A wider perspective. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 267–291). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd edn.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. Matson, J. L., & Sweizy, N. (1994). Social skills training with autistic children. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Autism in children and adults: Etiology, assessment and intervention (pp. 241–260). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  40. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Books for Young Readers.Google Scholar
  41. Merrit, D., & Liles, B. (1989). Narrative analysis: Clinical applications of story generation and story-retelling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 429–438.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1993). Is autism an amnesic disorder? Evidence from the California Verbal Learning Test. Neuropsychology, 7, 209–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Ennis, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 904–913.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language disorder, and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 227–251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R. M., Klin, A., McMahon, W. M., Minshew, N., Munson, J. A., Pennington, B. F., Rogers, S. J., Spence, M. A., Tager-Flusberg, H., Volkmar, F. R., & Wrathall, D. (2004). Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with Autistic Disorder: Evidence from the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 139–150.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Pearlman-Avinion, S., & Eviatar, Z. (2001). Narrative analysis in developmental, social, and linguistic pathologies: Dissociation between emotional and informational language use. Brain and Cognition, 48, 494–499.Google Scholar
  48. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Phelps-Terasaki, D., & Phelps-Gunn, T. (1992). Test of pragmatic language. London: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  50. Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism, perspectives from theory and research (pp. 149–169). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  51. Renner, P., Klinger, L. G., & Klinger, M. R. (2000). Implicit and explicit memory in autism: Is autism an amnesic disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 327–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2, 430–450.Google Scholar
  54. Schopler, E., Mesibov, G. B., & Kunce, L. J. (1998). Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism? New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  55. Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (1995). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (3rd edn.). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  56. Shapiro, T., & Kapit, R. (1978). Linguistic negation in autistic and normal children. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research, 7, 337–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Sheskin, D. J. (2000). Handbook of parametric and nonparametric statistical procedures (2nd edn.). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
  58. Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  59. Strong, C. J. (1998). The strong narrative assessment procedure. Madison, WI: Thinking Publications.Google Scholar
  60. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). A psycholinguistic perspective on language development in the autistic child. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 92–115). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  61. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1995). ‘Once upon a ribbit’: Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 45–59.Google Scholar
  62. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). Understanding the language and communicative impairments in autism. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 185–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 75–80.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 241–256.Google Scholar
  65. Thorndike, R. L., Hager, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale (4th edn.). Chicago: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
  66. Trabasso, T., Secco, T., & van den Broek, P. (1984). Causal cohesion and story coherence. In H. Mandl, N. L. Stein, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 83–111). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  67. Trabasso, T., & Sperry, L. L. (1985). Causal relatedness and importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 595–611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Trabasso, T., & van den Broek, P. (1985). Causal thinking and representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612–630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Twachtman-Cullen, D. (1998). Language and communication in high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. In E. Schopler, G. B. Mesibov, & L. J. Kunce (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism? (pp. 199–225). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  70. van den Broek, P., Kremer, K., Lynch, J. S., Kendeou, P., White, M. J., Butler, J., & Wenner, J. A. (2003). Comprehension skills in preschool children and their relation to later reading comprehension. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
  71. Wahlberg, T. J. (2001). Text comprehension in individuals with autism: The ability to comprehend written text in high-functioning individuals with autism. Dissertation Abstracts International, 62, 3818.Google Scholar
  72. Whirley, K. S., Lemberger, C. C., Ciarimboli, E. B., Estis, M., Lorch, E. P., & Trauth, D. (2003). What's the story? Narrative recall in children with ADHD. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
  73. Wolman, C., van den Broek, P., Lorch, R. F., Jr. (1997). Effects of causal structure on immediate and delayed story recall by children with mild mental retardation, children with learning disabilities, and children without disabilities. Journal of Special Education, 30, 439–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Young, E. C., Diehl, J. J., Morris, D., Hyman, S. L., & Bennetto, L. (2005). Pragmatic Language Disorders in Children with Autism: The use of two formal tests to distinguish affected children from controls. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 62–72.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joshua J. Diehl
    • 1
  • Loisa Bennetto
    • 1
  • Edna Carter Young
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Clinical & Social Sciences in PsychologyUniversity of RochesterRochesterUSA
  2. 2.Department of SurgeryUniversity of Rochester School of Medicine & DentistryRochesterUSA

Personalised recommendations