Skip to main content
Log in

Educating with Brain, Body and World Together

  • Published:
Interchange Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It seems reasonable to suppose that educational practices should be informed by philosophical and scientific understandings of the character and operation of mental processes. Clark and Chambers’ 1998 ‘The Extended Mind’ is a seminal paper in the philosophy of mind, but has received limited attention by educational researchers. Their Extended Mind Theory (EMT) provocatively claims that the assumption the mind is restricted to the head is unjustified, and that objects in the environment can function as parts of the mind. Clark and Chamber wrote that the human organism can be “linked with an external entity in a two-way interaction, creating a coupled system that can be seen as a cognitive system in its own right”. So, EMT challenges the assumption that the demarcation of skin and skull determines the boundaries of cognition, and demands a radical rethink of the nature of learning. This paper introduces and critiques EMT, initially by way of a discussion of embodiment, an intermediate step in the argument towards extension. It highlights inconsistencies in Clark and Chambers’ account, but concludes that the concept of a mind that extends into and interacts with its body and environment is both plausible and helpful.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, F., & Aizawa, K. (2001). The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology,14(1), 43–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aizawa, K., & Adams, F. (2005). Defending non-derived content. Philosophical Psychology,18(6), 661–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aydin, C. (2015). The artifactual mind. Phenomenology and Cognitive Science,14, 73–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aydin, C., Woge, M. G., & Verbeek, P. P. (2018). Technological environmentality: Conceptualizing technology as a mediating milieu. Philosophy & Technology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13347-018-0309-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, R. P. (2017). Science, pseudoscience and exercise neuroscience (pp. 335–359). In R. Meeusen, S. Schaefer, P. Tomporowski, & R. P. Bailey (Eds.), Physical activity and educational achievement: Insights from exercise neuroscience. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, R. P., Howells, K., & Glibo, I. (2017). Education 2030: Reviews of the outcomes of school physical education. Paris: OECD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrow, R. (2008). Education and the body: Prolegomena. British Journal of Educational Studies,56(3), 272–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of the mind. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bax, C. (2009). Subjectivity after Wittgenstein: Wittgenstein’s embodied and embedded subject and the debate about the death of man. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Institute for Logic, Language and Computation, University of Amsterdam.

  • Bleazby, J. (2015). Why some school subjects have a higher status than others: The epistemology of the traditional curriculum hierarchy. Oxford Review of Education,41(5), 671–689.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review,67, 380–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2005). Intrinsic content, active memory and the extended mind. Analysis,65(285), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2010). Memento’s revenge. In R. Menary (Ed.), The extended mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis,58(1), 7–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claxton, G. (2015). Intelligence in the flesh. New Haven, CN: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cope, E., & Bailey, R. P. (2016). A review of research and evidence on factors that impact on children’s sport and physical activity behaviours and attitudes. London: Sport England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. New York: Little, Brown and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1938). Experience and education. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (2009). Where is my mind. London Review of Books,31(3), 13–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2013). The socially extended mind. Cognitive Systems Research,25, 4–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist interventions: Rethinking the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, S. (2015). Mind change: How digital technologies are leaving their mark on our brains. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2000). The perception of the environment. Essays in livelihood, dwelling and skill. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiran, A. H., & Verbeek, P. P. (2010). Trusting our selves to technology. Knowledge, Technology & Policy,23(3–4), 409–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kontra, C., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Embodied learning across the life span. Topics in Cognitive Science,4(4), 731–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1993). We have never been modern. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leder, D. (1990). The absent body. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, N. (2007). Neuroethics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macdonald, C. (2007). Introspection and authoritative self-knowledge. Erkenntnis,67(2), 355–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2004). The cognitive basis of material engagement: Where brain, body and culture conflate. In E. DeMarrais, C. Gosden, & C. Renfrew (Eds.), Rethinking materiality: The engagement of mind with the material world. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (2006). Attacking the bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology,19, 329–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (Ed.). (2010). The extended mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1965). Phenomenology of perception (Trans. C. Smith). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

  • Mullender-Wijnsma, M. J., Hartman, E., de Greeff, J. W., Bosker, R. J., Doolaard, S., & Visscher, C. (2015). Improving academic performance of school-age children by physical activity in the classroom: 1-year programme evaluation. Journal of School Health,85(6), 365–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, U., & Newman, J. (2008). The body in action (pp. 313–342). In W. Overton, U. Müller, & J. Newman (Eds), Developmental perspectives on embodiment and consciousness. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowell, A., & Davidson, I. (Eds.). (2010). Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition. Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2016). Education at a glance 2016: OECD indicators. Paris: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. (2015). Processes, relations, and relational-developmental-systems. In R. M. Lerner, W. F. Overton, & P. C. Molenaar (Eds), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Volume 1: Theory and method (7th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D.N. (1993) Person-plus: a distributed view of thinking and learning. In G. Salomon. (Ed). Distributed cognitions. Psychological and educational considerations (pp. 88–110). Cambridge, USA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, R. S. (1966). Ethics and Education. London: Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M. A. (2002). Heidegger, education, and modernity. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K.R. (1994). The autonomy of world 3. In M. A. Notturno (Ed.), Knowledge and the body-mind problem: In defence of interaction pp. (24–46). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prensky, M. (2013). Our brains extended. Educational Leadership,70(6), 22–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1987). The many faces of realism. LaSalle, IL: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, M. (2004). The mind at work: Valuing the intelligence of the American worker. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, J. (in press). Conversations on embodiment across higher education. London: Routledge.

  • Rozemond, M. (2009). Descartes's dualism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheler, M. (1916/1954). Der Formalismus in der Ethik und die materiale Wertethik. Bern: Francke.

  • Schwitzgebel, E. (2015). Belief. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2015 Edition). Retrieved July 7, 2017, from: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2015/entries/belief.

  • Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,3, 417–457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1992). The rediscovery of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S., & Fernbach, P. (2017). The knowledge illusion: Why we never think alone. New York: Riverhead Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J., Harris, C. B., Keil, P. G., & Barnier, A. J. (2010). The psychology of memory, extended cognition, and socially distributed remembering. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences,9(4), 521–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, Ü., Okuyan, M., Albayrak, T., & Akgun, A. (2003). Sex differences in verbal and spatial ability reconsidered in relation to body size lung volume, and sex hormones. Perceptual and Motor Skills,96, 1347–1360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teese, R., & Polesel, J. (2003). Undemocratic schooling: Equity and quality in mass secondary education in Australia. Melbourne: Melbourne University Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tondeur, J., van Braak, J., Ertmer, P. A., & Ottenbreit-Leftwich, A. (2017). Understanding the relationship between teachers’ pedagogical beliefs and technology use in education. Educational Technology Research and Development,65(3), 555–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuk, M. A., Trampe, D., & Warlop, L. (2011). Inhibitory spillover: Increased urination urgency facilitates impulse control in unrelated domains. Psychological Science,22(5), 627–633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (2017). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, M. (2011). Thinking beyond the brain: Educating and building from the standpoint of extended cognition. Computational Culture, 1.

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolfolk Hoy, A. and Murphy, P.K. (2001) Teaching educational psychology to the implicit mind (pp. 145–186). In B. Torff, & R. Sternberg (Eds), Understanding and teaching the intuitive mind. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard Bailey.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bailey, R. Educating with Brain, Body and World Together. Interchange 51, 277–291 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10780-020-09390-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10780-020-09390-5

Keywords

Navigation