Abstract
Few primates are characterized by strict territoriality, pair-bonding, and loud, complex vocalizations. Gibbons (Hylobatidae) are amongst them, with mated pairs of most species performing duet songs that may strengthen pair-bonds and/or defend a territory. Gibbon duets often are audible well beyond a pair’s territory boundaries, suggesting that they function in intergroup signaling not just with close neighbors, but also with distant neighbors, yet no clear evidence for communication between distant neighbors has been found and direct tests of long-distance signaling are lacking. We studied vocal interactions in wild, white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) over two 10-day collection periods, from 340 recording hours on passive acoustic monitors at Khao Yai National Park, Thailand. Singing animals exhibited rapid control over vocal output (call inhibition) in response to short climax-coda components of distant pairs’ songs, supporting the intergroup long-distance signaling hypothesis. These climax-coda sequences are the only occasions in the species’ repertoire when phrases from both members of a pair regularly and unambiguously occur in rapid succession, suggesting a signaling of pair-bonded status to other groups, because only pair-bonded individuals sing in this way together. The call inhibition may allow distant receivers to estimate the location of a singing group’s territory, to identify potential mates or rivals, and may provide information that facilitates natal dispersal of mature offspring. We also provide evidence to support the hypothesis that the great call may have evolved a lengthy onset to allow distant receivers to detect the great call sequence before its climax-coda components occur.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Anandam, M. V., Groves, C. P., Molur, S., Rawson, B. M., Ricardson, M. C., & Roose, C. (2013). Species accounts of hylobatidae. In Mittermeier, R.a., Rylands, a.B., Wilson, D.E. (Eds). Handbook of the mammals of the world, 3. Primates. Lynx. (pp. 778–791).
Anikin, A. (2020). The link between auditory salience and emotion intensity. Cognition and Emotion, 34(6), 1246–1259. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2020.1736992.
Asensio, N., Brockelman, W. Y., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. H. (2014). White-handed gibbon (Hylobates lar) core area use over a short-time scale. Biotropica, 46, 461–469.
Barelli, C., Mastudaira, K., Roos, C., Wolf, T., Heistermann, M., Hodges, K., Takafumi, I., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. H. (2013). Extrapair paternity confirmed in wild white-handed gibbons. American Journal of Primatology, 75(12), 1185–1195. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22180.
Barker, N. K. S., Dabelsteen, T., & Mennill, D. J. (2009). Degradation of male and female rufous-and-white wren songs in a tropical forest: Effects of sex, perch height, and habitat. Behaviour, 146(8), 1093–1122. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853909X406446.
Brockelman, W. Y., & Schilling, D. (1984). Inheritance of stereotyped gibbon calls. Nature, 312, 634–636 https://www.nature.com/articles/312634a0.
Brockelman, W. Y., & Ali, R. (1987). Methods of surveying and sampling forest primate populations. In marsh, C. W. & Mittermeier, R. a. (Eds.), conservation in the tropical rainforest. Alan R. Liss, (pp. 23–62).
Brockelman, W. Y., & Srikosamatara, S. (1993). Estimation of density of gibbon groups by use of loud songs. American Journal of Primatology, 29, 93–108. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350290203.
Brockelman, W. Y., Reichard, U., Treesucon, U., & Raemaekers, J. J. (1998). Dispersal, pair formation and social structure in gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 42, 329–339 https://www.jstor.org/stable/4601460.
Brown, C. H., Gomez, R., & Waser, P. M. (1995). Old world monkey vocalizations: Adaptation to the local habitat. Animal Behaviour, 50, 945–961. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(95)80096-4.
Center for Conservation Bioacoustics website. (2017). Retrieved 7/7/21 from: https://www.birds.cornell.edu/ccb/swift-frequently-asked-questions/
Charlton, B. D., Owen, M. A., & Swaisgood, R. R. (2019). Coevolution of vocal signal characteristics and hearing sensitivity in forest mammals. Nature Communications, 10, 2778. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-10768-y.
Cheyne, S. M., Chivers, D. J., & Sugardjito, J. (2007). Covariation in the great calls of rehabilitant and wild gibbons Hylobates agilis albibarbis. Raffles Bulletin of Zoology, 55, 201–207.
Cheyne, S. M. (2008). Effects of meteorology, astronomical variables, location and human disturbance on the singing apes: Hylobates albibarbis. American Journal of Primatology., 70, 386–392. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20502.
Chivers, D. J. (1976). Communication within and between family groups of siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus). Behaviour, 57(1), 116–135.
Clarke, E. (2010). The vocalisations and anti-predatory behaviour of wild white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) in Khao Yai National Park, ThailandDoctoral thesis. University of St Andrews.
Clink, D. J., Lau, A. R., & Bales, K. L. (2019). Age-related changes and vocal convergence in titi monkey duet pulses. Behaviour, 156(15), 1471–1494. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003575.
Clink, D. J., Tasirin, J. S., & Klinck, H. (2020). Vocal individuality and rhythm in male and female duet contributions of a nonhuman primate. Current Zoology, 66(2), 173–186. https://doi.org/10.1093/cz/zoz035.
Clink, D. J., & Klinck, H. (2020). Unsupervised acoustic classification of individual gibbon females and the implications for passive acoustic monitoring. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 12, 328–341. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13520.
Cowlishaw, G. (1992). Song function in gibbons. Behaviour, 121(1), 131–153.
Cowlishaw, G. (1996). Sexual selection and information content in gibbon song bouts. Ethology, 102(2), 272–284. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01125.x.
Crunchant, A. S., Borchers, D., Kühl, H., & Piel, A. (2020). Listening and watching: Do camera traps or acoustic sensors more efficiently detect wild chimpanzees in an open habitat? Methods in Ecology Evolution, 00, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13362.
Dallmann, R., & Geissmann, T. (2009). Individual and geographical variability in the songs of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch) on Java, Indonesia. In S. Lappan & D. J. Whittacker (Eds.), The gibbons: New perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology (pp. 91–110). Springer.
Delgado, R. A., Lameira, A. R., Ross, M. D., Husson, S. J., Morrogh-Bernard, H., & Wich, S. (2010). In S. In Wich, S. Atmoko, T. Setia, & C. Van Schaik (Eds.), Geographical variation in orangutan long calls (pp. 215–224). Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation. Oxford University Press.
Eckhardt, N., Polanský, L., & Boesch, C. (2015). Spatial cohesion of adult male chimpanzees (pan troglodytes verus) in tai National Park, Côte d'Ivoire. American Journal of Primatology, 77(2), 125–134. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22316.
Enari, H., Enari, H. S., Okuda, K., Maruyama, T., & Okuda, K. N. (2019). An evaluation of the efficiency of passive acoustic monitoring in detecting deer and primates in comparison with camera traps. Ecological Indicators, 98, 753–762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolind.2018.11.062.
Ey, E., & Fischer, J. (2009). The "acoustic adaptation hypothesis" - a review of the evidence from birds, anurans and mammals. Bioacoustics., 19(1-2), 21–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2009.9753613.
Fan, P., Xiao, W., Huo, S., & Jiang, X. (2009). Singing behavior and singing functions of black-crested gibbons (Nomascus concolor jingdongensis) at Mt. Wuliang, Central Yunnan. China. American Journal of Primatology., 71(7), 539–547. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20686.
Fan, P., He, K., Chen, X., Ortiz, A., Zhang, B., Zhao, C., Li, Y., Zhang, H., Kimock, C., Wang, W., Groves, C., Turvey, S., Roos, C., Helgen, K., & Jiang, X. (2017). Description of a new species of hoolock gibbon (primates: Hylobatidae) based on integrative taxonomy. American Journal of Primatology, 79(5). https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22631.
Fichtel, C., & Manser, M. (2010). Vocal communication in social groups. In P. Kappeler (Ed.), Animal behaviour: Evolution and mechanisms (pp. 29–54). Springer.
Gamba, M., Valente, D., De Gregorio, C., & Zanoli, A. (2019). Song structure and sex-specific features in the indris. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 145, 1919. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5101970.
Geissmann, T. (1984). Inheritance of Song Parameters in the Gibbon Song, Analysed in 2 Hybrid Gibbons (Hylobates pileatus × H. lar). Folia Primatologica, 42(3–4), 216–235. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156165.
Geissmann, T. (1993). Evolution of communication in gibbons (Hylobatidae), [doctoral dissertation, anthropological institute, Philosoph, faculty II, Zürich University]. http://www.gibbons.de/main/theses/pdf_files/1993geissmann.pdf.
Geissmann, T. (1999). Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: II. Testing the pair-bonding hypothesis during a partner exchange. Behaviour, 136(8), 1005–1039. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853999501694.
Geissmann, T. (2000). In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), Gibbon songs and human music from an evolutionary perspective. In the origins of music (pp. 103–123). MIT Press.
Geissmann, T. (2002). Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society., 77(1), 57–76. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1464793101005826.
Geissmann, T., & Mutschler, T. (2006). Diurnal distribution of loud calls in sympatric wild indris (Indri indri) and ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata): Implications for call functions. Primates, 47(4), 393–396. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-006-0189-5.
Geissmann, T., & Orgeldinger, M. (2000). The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs. Hylobates syndactylus. Animal Behaviour, 60(6), 805–809. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1540.
Haimoff, E. H. (1986). Convergence in the duetting of monogamous Old World primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 15(1), 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(86)80065-3.
Heffner, R. S. (2004). Primate hearing from a mammalian perspective. The Anatomical Record, 281A(1), 1111–1122. https://doi.org/10.1002/ar.a.20117.
Heinicke, S., Kalan, A. K., Wagner, O. J., Mundry, R., Lukashevich, H., & Kühl, H. S. (2015). Assessing the performance of a semi-automated acoustic monitoring system for primates. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 6, 753–763. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12384.
Jürgens, U., & Hage, S. R. (2007). On the role of the reticular formation in vocal pattern generation. Behavioural Brain Research, 182(2), 308–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2006.11.027.
Kalan, A. K., Piel, A. K., Mundry, R., Wittig, R. M., Boesch, C., & Kühl, H. S. (2016). Passive acoustic monitoring reveals group ranging and territory use: A case study of wild chimpanzees (pan troglodytes). Frontiers in Zoology, 13(34). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-016-0167-8.
Keith, S. A., Waller, M. S., & Geissmann, T. (2009). Vocal diversity of Kloss’s gibbons (Hylobates klossii) in the Mentawai Islands, Indonesia. In Lappan S., & Whittacker D. J. (Eds.) the gibbons: New perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology, springer (pp. 51–71). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88604-6_4.
Koda, H., Lemasson, A., Oyakawa, C., Rizaldi, P. J., & Masataka, N. (2013). Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS One, 8(8), e71432. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071432.
Langmore, N. E. (1998). Functions of duet and solo songs of female birds. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 13(4), 136–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(97)01241-X.
Ma, C., Trinh-Dinh, H., Nguyen, V., Le, T., Le, V., Le, H., Yang, J., Zhang, Z., & Fan, P. (2020). Transboundary conservation of the last remaining population of the cao vit gibbon Nomascus nasutus. Oryx, 54(6), 776–783. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0030605318001576.
Marten, K., Quine, D., & Marler, P. (1977). Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization: II. Tropical forest habitats. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2(3), 291–302 http://www.jstor.org/stable/4599137.
Martin, R. D. (1986). Primates: A definition. In B. Wood, L. Martin, & P. Andrews (Eds.), Major topics in primate and human evolution (pp. 1–31). Cambridge University Press.
Martínez, A. E., Parra, E., Gomez, J. P., & Vredenburg, V. T. (2021). Shared predators between primate groups and mixed species bird flocks: The potential for forest-wide eavesdropping networks. Oikos. https://doi.org/10.1111/oik.08274.
Masters, J. C. (1991). Loud calls of Galago crassicaudatus and G. garnettii and their relation to habitat structure. Primates, 32(2), 153–167. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381173.
McGregor, P. K. (2005). Animal communication networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511610363
McGregor, P. K., & Dabelsteen, T. (1996). Communication networks. In D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller (Eds.), Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds (pp. 409–425). Cornell University Press.
Mitani, J. C. (1984). The behavioral regulation of monogamy in gibbons (Hylobates muelleri). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology., 15(3), 225–229. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00292979.
Mitani, J. C. (1985a). Gibbon song duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour, 92(1-2), 59–96. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853985X00389.
Mitani, J. C. (1985b). Responses of gibbons (Hylobates muelleri) to self, neighbor, and stranger song duets. International Journal of Primatology, 6, 193–200. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02693653.
Mittermeier, R. A., Rylands A. B., & Wilson, D. E. (2013). Handbook of the mammals of the world: 3. Primtes. . ISBN: 978-84-96553-89-7
Myers, N., Mittermeier, R., Mittermeier, C., da Fonseca, G., & Kent, J. (2000). Biodiversity hotspots for conservation priorities. Nature, 403, 853–858. https://doi.org/10.1038/35002501.
Nietsch, A. (1999). Duet vocalizations among different populations of Sulawesi tarsiers. International Journal of Primatology, 20, 567–583. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020342807709.
O’Brien, T. G., Kinnaird, M. F., Nurcahyo, A., Iqbal, M., & Rusmanto, M. (2004). Abundance and distribution of sympatric gibbons in a threatened Sumatran rainforest. International Journal of Primatology, 25(2), 267–284. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:IJOP.0000019152.83883.1c.
Palombit, R. (1994). Extra-pair copulations in a monogamous ape. Animal Behaviour, 47, 721–723.
Pérez-Granados, C., & Schuchmann, K. L. (2021). Passive acoustic monitoring of the diel and annual vocal behavior of the black and gold howler monkey. American Journal of Primatology., 83, e23241. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23241.
Pijanowski, B. C., Farina, A., Gage, S. H., Dumyahn, S. L., & Krause, B. L. (2011). What is soundscape ecology? An introduction and overview of an emerging new science. Landscape Ecology, 26, 1213–1232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10980-011-9600-8.
Pollock, J. (1986). The song of the indris (indri; primates: Lemuroidea): Natural history, form, and function. International Journal of Primatology, 7(3), 225–264. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02736391.
Raemaekers, J. J., Raemaekers, P. M., & Haimoff, E. H. (1984). Loud calls of the gibbon (Hylobates lar): Repertoire, organization and context. Behaviour, 91, 146–189. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853984X00263.
Raemaekers, P. M., & Raemaekers, J. J. (1985a). Long-range vocal interactions between groups of gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behaviour, 95(1/2), 26–44. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853985X00037.
Raemaekers, J. J., & Raemaekers, P. M. (1985b). Field playback of loud calls to gibbons (Hylobates lar): Territorial, sex-specific and species-specific responses. Animal Behaviour, 33, 481–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80071-3.
Reby, D., & Charlton, B. D. (2012). Attention grabbing in red deer sexual calls. Animal Cognition, 15, 265–270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0451-0.
Reichard, U. (1995). Extra-pair copulations in a monogamous gibbon (Hylobates lar). Ethology, 100, 99–112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1995.tb00319.x.
Reichard, U., & Sommer, V. (1997). Group encounters in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar): Agonism, affiliation, and the concept of infanticide. Behaviour, 134, 1135–1174 https://www.jstor.org/stable/4535499.
Reichard, U. H. (2009). Social organization and mating system of Khao Yai white-handed gibbons, 1992–2006. In S. Lappan & D. J. Whittacker (Eds.), The gibbons: New perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology (pp. 347–384). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88604-6_17.
Reichard, U. H., Ganpanakngan, M., & Barelli, C. (2012). White-handed gibbons of Khao Yai: Social flexibility, reproductive strategies, and a slow life history. In P. M. Kappeler & D. Watts (Eds.), Long-term field studies of primates (pp. 237–258). Springer.
Reichard, U. H., Barelli, C., Hirai, H., & Nowak, M. G. (2016). The evolution of gibbons and siamang. In Reichard, U.H., Hirai, H., & Barelli, C. (Eds.), evolution of gibbons and siamang phylogeny, morphology, and cognition, (pp. 3–41). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-5614-2_1
Riede, T. (2014). Rat ultrasonic vocalization shows features of a modular behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(20), 6874–6878. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0262-14.2014.
Robinson, J., & Redford, K. (1986). Body size, diet, and population density of neotropical forest mammals. The American Naturalist, 128(5), 665–680.
Ryan, M. J., & Cummings, M. E. (2013). Perceptual biases and mate choice. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 44(1), 437–459. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901.
Sabatini, V., & Ruiz-Miranda, C. R. (2010). Does the golden lion tamarin, Leontopithecus rosalia (primates: Callitrichidae), select a location within the forest strata for long distance communication? Zoologia, 27(2), 179–183. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1984-46702010000200004.
Sobroza, T. V., Gordo, M., Barnett, A. P. A., Boubli, J. P., & Spironello, W. R. (2021a). Parapatric pied and red-handed tamarin responses to congeneric and conspecific calls. Acta Oecologica, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actao.2020.103688.
Sobroza, T. V., Gordo, M., Pequeno, P. A. C. L., Dunn, J. C., Spironello, W. R., Rabelo, R. M., & Barnett, A. P. A. (2021b). Convergent character displacement in sympatric tamarin calls (Saguinus spp.). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 75, 88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-021-03028-x.
Thalmann, U., Geissmann, T., Simona, A., & Mutschler, T. (1993). The indris of Anjanaharibe-Sud, northeastern Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology, 14, 357–381. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02192772.
Sugai, L. S. M., Silva, T. S. F., Ribeiro, J. W., & Llusia, D. (2019). Terrestrial passive acoustic monitoring: Review and perspectives. BioScience, 69, 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biy147.
van Schaik, C. P., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1990). The evolution of monogamy in large primates: A new hypothesis and some crucial tests. Behaviour 115(½), 30–62. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4534885
Terleph, T. A., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. H. (2015). The Lar gibbon (Hylobates lar) great call reveals individual caller identity. American Journal of Primatology, 77, 811–821. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22406.
Terleph, T. A., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. H. (2016). Age related decline in female Lar gibbon great call performance suggests that call features correlate with physical condition. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 16, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0578-8.
Terleph, T. A., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. H. (2018a). Male white-handed gibbons flexibly time duet contributions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-017-2432-z.
Terleph, T. A., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. H. (2018b). An analysis of white-handed gibbon male song reveals speech-like phrases. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.23451.
Thomas, L., & Marques, T. A. (2012). Passive acoustic monitoring for estimating animal density. Acoustics Today, 8(3), 35–44. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4753915.
Thorpe, W. H. (1972). Duetting and antiphonal song in birds. Behaviour (Suppl.), 18, 1–193 https://www.jstor.org/stable/30039182.
Traeholt, C., Bonthoeun, R., Virak, C., Samuth, M., & Vutthin, S. (2006). Song activity of the pileated gibbon, Hylobates pileatus, in Cambodia. Primate Conservation, 21, 139–144. https://doi.org/10.1896/0898-6207.21.1.139.
Van Belle, S., & Estrada, A. (2020). The influence of loud calls on intergroup spacing mechanisms in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). International Journal of Primatology, 41, 265–286. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-019-00121-x.
Waser, P. M., & Brown, C. H. (1986). Habitat acoustics and primate communications. American Journal of Primatology, 10(2), 135–154. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350100205.
Weber, J., Peterson, B., & Hoekstra, H. (2013). Discrete genetic modules are responsible for complex burrow evolution in Peromyscus mice. Nature, 493, 402–405. https://doi.org/10.1038/493312a.
Wich, S. A., & Nunn, C. L. (2002). Do male "long-distance calls" function in mate defense? A comparative study of long-distance calls in primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 52(6), 474–484 https://www.jstor.org/stable/4602170.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded in part by a URCG grant (Sacred Heart University) to Dr. Terleph. Thanks to the National Research Council of Thailand for research permits, and the Department of the National Parks, Wildlife, and Plant Conservation for park permits.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
TT designed the study and analyzed the data. TT, CS, and UR collected the data. TT and UR wrote the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Additional information
Handling Editor: Joanna Setchell
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Terleph, T., Saralamba, C. & Reichard, U.H. Long-Distance Vocal Signaling in White-Handed Gibbons (Hylobates lar). Int J Primatol 43, 965–986 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-022-00312-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-022-00312-z