International Journal of Primatology

, Volume 37, Issue 4–5, pp 568–585 | Cite as

Acoustic and Temporal Variation in Gelada (Theropithecus gelada) Loud Calls Advertise Male Quality

  • Marcela E. Benítez
  • Aliza le Roux
  • Julia Fischer
  • Jacinta C. Beehner
  • Thore J. Bergman
Article

Abstract

Many animals rely on information from vocal signals to assess potential competitors and mates. For example, in primates, males use loud calls to assess rivals when the acoustic properties of the calls reliably indicate the condition or quality of the sender. Here, we investigate whether the loud calls of male geladas (Theropithecus gelada) function as a quality signal. Gelada males produce loud calls during ritualistic chases with rival males. Given the physically taxing nature of these displays, we hypothesize that variation in the acoustic properties of loud calls reliably signal male stamina or competitive ability. To test this hypothesis, we examined whether the acoustic properties of the gelada loud call varied in relation to individual, age, status, and exhaustion. Specifically, we examined 12 call parameters (e.g., fundamental frequency) and 3 bout parameters (e.g., number of calls per bout), that have been previously shown to vary across condition in male primates. We found that several acoustic features varied consistently across age and status such that males deemed higher quality in gelada society (e.g., high status) produced more calls per bout, produced calls that were lower in overall frequency measures, and exhibited a greater vocal range. In addition, we found that similar acoustic features varied with exhaustion; after a long chase event, males produced both fewer calls per bout and calls with higher spectral measures. Results from this study are consistent with the hypothesis that gelada loud calls are quality signals, contributing to the growing evidence that primates may use acoustic information to assess the quality of a rival or a potential mate.

Keywords

Acoustic analysis Mate choice Quality signal Rival assessment Sexual selection Theropithecus 

Supplementary material

10764_2016_9922_MOESM1_ESM.docx (224 kb)
ESM 1(DOCX 223 kb)

References

  1. Aich, H., Moos-Heilen, R., & Zimmermann, E. (1990). Vocalizations of adult gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada): acoustic structure and behavioural context. Folia Primatologica, International Journal of Primatology, 55(3), 109–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barelli, C., Mundry, R., Heistermann, M., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2013). Cues to androgens and quality in male gibbon songs. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e82748.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., et al. (2015). Package lme4. (http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/).
  4. Beehner, J. C., Gesquiere, L., Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Alberts, S. C., Altmann, J. (2015). Corrigendum to “Testosterone related to age and life-history stages in male baboons and geladas.” [Hormones and Behavior 56/4 (2009) 472–480]. Hormones and Behavior. Google Scholar
  5. Bergman, T. J. (2010). Experimental evidence for limited vocal recognition in a wild primate: implications for the social complexity hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 277, 3045–3053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bergman, T. J., & Sheehan, M. J. (2013). Social knowledge and signals in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 683–694.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bouchet, H., Blois-Heulin, C., Pellier, A. S., Zuberbühler, K., & Lemasson, A. (2012). Acoustic variability and individual distinctiveness in the vocal repertoire of red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 45–56.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Caselli, C. B., Mennill, D. J., Bicca-Marques, J. C., & Setz, E. Z. (2014). Vocal behavior of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons): acoustic properties and behavioral contexts of loud calls. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 788–800.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Delgado, R. A. (2006). Sexual selection in the loud calls of male primates: signal content and function. International Journal of Primatology, 27(1), 5–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1983). Structure of gelada baboon reproductive units. II. Social relationships between reproductive females. Animal Behaviour, 31, 556–564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1984). Reproductive decisions: An economic analysis of gelada baboon social strategies. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Dunbar, R. I. M., & Dunbar, P. (1975). Social dynamics of gelada baboons. In H. Kuhn, W. P. Luckett, C. R. Noback, A. H. Schultz, D. Starck, & F. S. Szalay (Eds.), Contributions to primatology. Basel: S. Karger.Google Scholar
  14. Erb, W. M., Hodges, J. K., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2013). Individual, contextual, and age-related acoustic variation in simakobu (Simias concolor) loud calls. PLoS ONE, 8, e83131.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Ey, E., Pfefferle, D., & Fischer, J. (2007). Do age- and sex-related variations reliably reflect body size in non-human primate vocalizations? A review. Primates, Journal of Primatology, 48, 253–267.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Fischer, J., Hammerschmidt, K., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2002). Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: influences of context, age, and individuality. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111, 1465–1474.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fischer, J., Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. (2004). Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: Acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 140–148.Google Scholar
  18. Fischer, J., Noser, R., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2013). Bioacoustic field research: a primer to acoustic analyses and playback experiments with primates. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 643–663.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Fitch, W. T. (1997). Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1213–1222.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (1995). Vocal production in nonhuman primates: acoustics, physiology, and functional constraints on “honest” advertisement. American Journal of Primatology, 37, 191–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gerhardt, H. C., Tanner, S. D., Corrigan, C. M., & Walton, H. C. (2000). Female preference functions based on call duration in the gray tree frog (Hyla versicolor). Behavioral Ecology, 11, 663–669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hall, K. R. L., & DeVore, I. (1965). Baboon social behavior. In I. DeVore (Ed.), Primate behavior: Field studies of monkeys and apes (pp. 53–110). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  23. Holt, M. M., Noren, D. P., Dunkin, R. C., & Williams, T. M. (2015). Vocal performance affects metabolic rate in dolphins: implications for animals communicating in noisy environments. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 218, 1647–1654.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Inoue, M. (1988). Age gradations in vocalization and body weight in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Folia Primatologica, 51, 76–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kawai, M., Ohsawa, H., Mori, U., & Dunbar, R. (1983). Social organization of gelada baboons: Social units and definitions. Primates, Journal of Primatology, 24, 13–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., Fischer, J., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Loud calls as indicators of dominance in male baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53, 374–384.Google Scholar
  27. le Roux, & Bergman. (2012). Indirect rival assessment in a social primate, Theropithecus gelada. Animal Behaviour, 83, 249–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. McComb, K. (1991). Female choice for high roaring rates in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Animal Behaviour, 41, 79–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Méndez-Cárdenas, M., Randrianambinina, B., Rabesandratana, A., Rasoloharijaona, S., & Zimmermann, E. (2008). Geographic variation in loud calls of sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ssp.) and their implications for conservation. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 828–838.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Mori, U. (1979a). Social structure of gelada baboons. In M. Kawai (Ed.), Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons (Vol. 16, pp. 243–247). Basel: S. Karger.Google Scholar
  31. Mori, U. (1979b). Unit formation and the emergence of a new leader. In M. Kawai (Ed.), Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons (Vol. 16, pp. 155–181). Basel: S. Karger.Google Scholar
  32. Mougeot, F., Irvine, J. R., Seivwright, L., Redpath, S. M., & Piertney, S. (2004). Testosterone, immunocompetence, and honest sexual signaling in male red grouse. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 930–937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mundry, R., & Fischer, J. (1998). Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often leads to incorrect p values: Examples from animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 56, 256–259.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Mundry, R., & Sommer, C. (2007). Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative. Animal Behaviour, 74, 965–976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Neumann, C., Assahad, G., Hammerschmidt, K., Perwitasari-Farajallah, D., & Engelhardt, A. (2010). Loud calls in male crested macaques, Macaca nigra: a signal of dominance in a tolerant species. Animal Behaviour, 79, 187–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pfefferle, D., & Fischer, J. (2006). Sounds and size: identification of acoustic variables that reflect body size in hamadryas baboons, Papio hamadryas. Animal Behaviour, 72, 43–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Reby, D., McComb, K., Cargnelutti, B., Darwin, C., Fitch, W. T., & Clutton-Brock, T. (2005). Red deer stags use formants as assessment cues during intrasexual agonistic interactions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 272(1566), 941–947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Riede, T., Arcadi, A. C., & Owren, M. J. (2007). Nonlinear acoustics in the pant hoots of common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): vocalizing at the edge. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121, 1758–1767.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Snyder-Mackler, N., Alberts, S. C., & Bergman, T. J. (2014). The socio-genetics of a complex society: female gelada relatedness patterns mirror association patterns in a multilevel society. Molecular Ecology, 23, 6179–6191.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Spillmann, B., Dunkel, L. P., Van Noordwijk, M. A., Amda, R. N., Lameira, A. R., et al. (2010). Acoustic properties of long calls given by flanged male orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) reflect both individual identity and context. Ethology, 116, 385–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Thomas, R. J. (2002). The costs of singing in nightingales. Animal Behaviour, 63, 959–966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Titze, I. R., & Riede, T. (2010). A cervid vocal fold model suggests greater glottal efficiency in calling at high frequencies. PLoS Computational Biology, 6, e1000897.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Vehrencamp, S. (2000). Handicap, index, and conventional signal elements of bird song. In Y. Espmark, T. Amundsen, & G. Rosenqvist (Eds.), Animal signals: signalling and signal design in animal communication (pp. 227–300). Trondheim: Tapir Academic Press.Google Scholar
  45. Wagner, W. E. (1992). Deceptive or honest signalling of fighting ability? A test of alternative hypotheses for the function of changes in call dominant frequency by male cricket frogs. Animal Behaviour, 44, 449–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Ward, S., Speakman, J. R., & Slater, P. J. (2003). The energy cost of song in the canary, Serinus canaria. Animal Behaviour, 66, 893–902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wich, S. A., Van der Post, D. J., Heistermann, M., Möhle, U., Van Hooff, J., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2003). Life-phase related changes in male loud call characteristics and testosterone levels in wild Thomas langurs. International Journal of Primatology, 24, 1251–1265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marcela E. Benítez
    • 1
    • 2
  • Aliza le Roux
    • 4
  • Julia Fischer
    • 5
  • Jacinta C. Beehner
    • 1
    • 2
  • Thore J. Bergman
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of MichiganAnn ArborUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MichiganAnn ArborUSA
  3. 3.Department of Ecology and Evolutionary BiologyUniversity of MichiganAnn ArborUSA
  4. 4.Department of Zoology and EntomologyUniversity of the Free StateQwaqwaSouth Africa
  5. 5.Cognitive Ethology LaboratoryGerman Primate CenterGöttingenGermany

Personalised recommendations