Female Competition over Core Areas in Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, Kibale National Park, Uganda

  • Sonya M. Kahlenberg
  • Melissa Emery Thompson
  • Richard W. Wrangham
Article

Abstract

Aggression is rare among wild female chimpanzees. However, in the Kanyawara chimpanzee community in Kibale National Park, Uganda, stable use of food-rich core areas is linked to increased reproductive success, suggesting that contest competition might occur over access to the highest-quality ranges. To examine this hypothesis, we studied aggression and dominance relationships among Kanyawara females during a 10-yr period that included the immigration of 5 females into the community. We tested 2 predictions: 1) that female-female aggression should intensify when immigrants enter the community because this is when core area access is determined and 2) that the quality of core areas should reflect relative female dominance relationships. In support of the first prediction, female-female aggression increased 4-fold when new immigrants were in the community, with rates peaking when there were multiple immigrants. This pattern was due primarily to aggression by resident mothers toward immigrants and featured coalitionary aggression, a rare behavior among female chimpanzees. In support of the second prediction, females occupying core areas high in foraging quality ranked high overall and higher than expected for their ages, whereas females occupying low-quality core areas were lower-ranking and ranked lower than expected for their ages. Together, the data indicate that though female aggression does not regularly occur in chimpanzees, contest competition continues to play an important role in determining long-term access to resources, an important correlate of reproductive success.

Keywords

chimpanzees dispersal dominance intrasexual aggression ranging 

References

  1. Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227–267. doi:10.1163/156853974X00534.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baker, K. C., & Smuts, B. B. (1994). Social relationships of female chimpanzees: Diversity between captive social groups. In R. W. Wrangham, W. C. McGrew, F. B. M. de Waal, & P. G. Heltne (Eds.), Chimpanzee Cultures (pp. 227–242). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Boesch, C. (1996). Social grouping in Taï chimpanzees. In W. C. McGrew, L. F. Marchant, & T. Nishida (Eds.), Great Ape Societies (pp. 101–113). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Boesch, C., & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioral ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bygott, D. (1979). Agonistic behaviour and dominance in wild chimpanzees. In D. Hamburg, & E. R. McCown (Eds.), The Great Apes (pp. 405–427). Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings.Google Scholar
  6. Chapais, B. (1992). The role of alliances in social inheritance of rank among female primates. In A. H. Harcourt, & F. B. M. de Waal (Eds.), Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals (pp. 29–59). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Chapman, C. A., & Wrangham, R. W. (1993). Range use of the forest chimpanzees of Kibale: Implications for the understanding of chimpanzee social organization. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 263–273. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350310403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: A cross-species comparison. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 257–333. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(95)00050-U.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Emery Thompson, M. (2005). Reproductive endocrinology of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): Methodological considerations and the role of hormones in sex and conception. American Journal of Primatology, 67, 137–158. doi:10.1002/ajp.20174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Emery Thompson, M., Kahlenberg, S. M., Gilby, I. C., & Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Core area quality is associated with variance in reproductive success among female chimpanzees at Kibale National Park. Animal Behaviour, 73, 501–512. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.09.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Emery Thompson, M., Newton-Fisher, N. E., & Reynolds, V. (2006). Probable community transfer of parous adult female chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 27, 1601–1617. doi:10.1007/s10764-006-9098-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Emery Thompson, M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2006). Comparison of sex differences in gregariousness in fission-fusion species: Reducing bias by standardizing for party size. In N. E. Newton-Fisher, H. Notman, V. Reynolds, & J. Paterson (Eds.), Primates of Western Uganda (pp. 209–226). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Emery Thompson, M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2008). Diet and reproductive function in wild female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Kibale National Park, Uganda. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 135, 171–181.Google Scholar
  14. Fawcett, K. A. (2000). Female Relationships and Food Availability in a Forest Community of Chimpanzees. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
  15. Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Greengrass, E. (2005). Sociability and dominance among female chimpanzees at Gombe. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Bristol, Bristol, U.K.Google Scholar
  17. Harcourt, A. H. (1987). Dominance and fertility among female primates. Journal of Zoology, 213, 471–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hasegawa, T. (1990). Sex differences in ranging patterns. In T. Nishida (Ed.), The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (pp. 99–114). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
  19. Jameson, K. A., Appleby, M. C., & Freeman, L. C. (1999). Finding an appropriate order for a hierarchy based on probabilistic dominance. Animal Behaviour, 57, 991–998. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.1077.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kahlenberg, S. M. (2006). Female-female competition and male sexual coercion in Kanyawara chimpanzees. Ph.D. thesis. Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  21. Kahlenberg, S. M., Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M. N., & Wrangham, R. W. (in press). Immigration costs for female chimpanzees and male protection as an immigrant counterstrategy to intrasexual aggression. Animal Behaviour.Google Scholar
  22. Knott, C. D. (2001). Female reproductive ecology of the apes: Implications for human evolution. In P. T. Ellison (Ed.), Reproductive ecology and human evolution (pp. 429–463). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  23. Koenig, A. (2002). Competition for resources and its behavioral consequences among female primates. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 759–783. doi:10.1023/A:1015524931226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lehmann, J., & Boesch, C. (2005). Bisexually bonded ranging in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57, 525–535. doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0891-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mitani, J. C., Watts, D. P., & Lwanga, J. S. (2002). Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size and composition. In C. Boesch, G. Hohmann, & L. Marchant (Eds.), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (pp. 102–111). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Muller, M. N. (2002). Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In C. Boesch, G. Hohmann, & L. Marchant (Eds.), Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos (pp. 112–124). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Muller, M. N. (2007). Chimpanzee violence: Femmes fatales. Current Biology, 17, R365–R366. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.037.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Muller, M. N., Emery Thompson, M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2006). Chimpanzee males prefer mating with old females. Current Biology, 16, 2234–2238. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.09.042.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Muller, M. N., Kahlenberg, S. M., Emery Thompson, M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Male coercion and costs of promiscuous mating for female chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 274, 1009–1014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Mundry, R., & Fischer, J. (1998). Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often lead to incorrect P values: Examples from Animal Behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 56, 256–259. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0756.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Murray, C. M. (2007). A method for assigning categorical rank in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) via the frequency of approaches. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 853–864. doi:10.1007/s10764-007-9164-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Murray, C. M., Eberly, L. E., & Pusey, A. E. (2006). Foraging strategies as a function of season and rank among wild female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioral Ecology, 17, 1020–1028. doi:10.1093/beheco/arl042.Google Scholar
  33. Murray, C. M., Mane, S. V., & Pusey, A. E. (2007). Dominance rank influences female space use in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Towards an ideal despotic distribution. Animal Behaviour, 74, 1795–1804.Google Scholar
  34. Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2004). Hierarchy and social status in Budongo chimpanzees. Primates, 45, 81–87. doi:10.1007/s10329-003-0064-6.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Nishida, T. (1989). Social interactions between resident and immigrant female chimpanzees. In P. G. Heltne, & L. A. Marquardt (Eds.), Understanding Chimpanzees (pp. 68–89). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Nishida, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T., & Takahata, Y. (1985). Group extinction and female transfer in wild chimpanzees in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 67, 284–301.Google Scholar
  37. Pepper, J., Mitani, J. C., & Watts, D. P. (1999). General gregariousness and specific social preferences among wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 20, 613–632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pusey, A. E. (1980). Inbreeding avoidance in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 28, 543–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Pusey, A. E. (1990). Behavioural changes at adolescence in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 115, 203–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Pusey, A. E., Murray, C., Wallauer, W., Wilson, M., Wroblewski, E., & Goodall, J. (2008). Severe aggression among female Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology, 29, this issue doi:10.1007/s10764-008-9281-6.
  41. Pusey, A. E., Oehlert, G. W., Williams, J. M., & Goodall, J. (2005). Influence of ecological and social factors on body mass of wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 26, 3–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pusey, A. E., & Packer, C. (1987). Dispersal and philopatry. In B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, & T. T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 250–266). Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Pusey, A. E., Williams, J. M., & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, 828–831.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sherry, D. S. (2002). Reproductive seasonality in chimpanzees and humans: Ultimate and proximate factors. Ph.D. thesis. Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  45. Skorupa, J. P. (1988). The effects of selective timber harvesting on rain-forest primates in Kibale Forest, Uganda. Ph.D. thesis. University of California, Davis.Google Scholar
  46. Sterck, E. H. M., Watts, D. P., & van Schaik, C. P. (1997). The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41, 291–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stumpf, R. (2007). Chimpanzees and bonobos: Diversity within and between species. In C. J. Campbell, A. Fuentes, K. C. Mackinnon, M. Panger, & S. K. Bearder (Eds.), Primates in perspective (pp. 321–344). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Townsend, S. W., Slocombe, K. E., Emery Thompson, M., & Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Female-led infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology, 17, 355–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. van Schaik, C. P. (1989). The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In R. A. Foley, & V. Standen (Eds.), Comparative socioecology (pp. 195–218). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  50. Williams, J. M. (2000). Female strategies and reasons for territoriality in chimpanzees: Lessons from three decades of research at Gombe. Ph.D. thesis, University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
  51. Williams, J. M., Liu, H.-Y., & Pusey, A. E. (2002a). Costs and benefits of grouping for female chimpanzees at Gombe. In C. Boesch, G. Hohmann, & L. Marchant (Eds.), Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos (pp. 192–203). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Williams, J. M., Oehlert, G. W., Carlis, J. V., & Pusey, A. E. (2004). Why do male chimpanzees defend a group range? Animal Behavior, 68, 523–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Williams, J. M., Pusey, A. E., Carlis, J. V., Farm, B. P., & Goodall, J. (2002b). Female competition and male territorial behaviour influence female chimpanzees’ ranging patterns. Animal Behaviour, 63, 347–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wilson, M. L. (2001). Imbalances of power: How chimpanzees respond to the threat of intergroup aggression. Ph.D. thesis. Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  55. Wilson, M. L., & Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 363–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wittig, R. M., & Boesch, C. (2003). Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Taї National Park. International Journal of Primatology, 24, 847–867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wrangham, R. W. (1979). Sex differences in chimpanzee dispersion. In D. Hamburg, & E. R. McCown (Eds.), The great apes (pp. 480–489). Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings.Google Scholar
  58. Wrangham, R. W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour, 75, 262–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wrangham, R. W. (2000). Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In P. M. Kappeler (Ed.), Primate males: Causes and consequences of variation in group composition (pp. 248–258). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Wrangham, R. W., Chapman, C. A., Clark-Arcadi, A. P., & Isabirye-Basuta, G. (1996). Social ecology of Kanyawara chimpanzees: Implications for understanding the costs of great ape groups. In W. C. McGrew, L. F. Marchant, & T. Nishida (Eds.), Great ape societies (pp. 45–57). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  61. Wrangham, R. W., Clark, A. P., & Isabirye-Basuta, G. (1992). Female social relationships and social organization of Kibale Forest chimpanzees. In T. Nishida, W. C. McGrew, P. Marler, M. Pickford, & F. B. M. de Waal (Eds.), Topics in primatology, vol. I: Human origins (pp. 81–98). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
  62. Wrangham, R. W., & Smuts, B. B. (1980). Sex differences in the behavioural ecology of chimpanzees in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility (Supplement), 28, 13–31.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sonya M. Kahlenberg
    • 1
    • 2
  • Melissa Emery Thompson
    • 1
    • 3
  • Richard W. Wrangham
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyHarvard UniversityCambridgeUSA
  2. 2.Department of BiologyBates CollegeLewistonUSA
  3. 3.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of New MexicoAlbuquerqueUSA

Personalised recommendations