Advertisement

CURRICULUM SEQUENCING AND THE ACQUISITION OF CLOCK-READING SKILLS AMONG CHINESE AND FLEMISH CHILDREN

  • Elise BurnyEmail author
  • Martin Valcke
  • Annemie Desoete
  • Johannes E. Hans Van Luit
Article

ABSTRACT

The present study addresses the impact of the curriculum on primary school children’s acquisition of clock-reading knowledge from analog and digital clocks. Focusing on Chinese and Flemish children’s clock-reading knowledge, the study is about whether the differences in sequencing of learning and instruction opportunities—as defined by the curriculum—result in differences in accuracy and development of clock-reading knowledge. By means of (1) an in-depth qualitative analysis of the Chinese and Flemish mathematics curriculum standards and textbooks and (2) a quantitative analysis of 11,743 students’ accuracy in reading the time, the current study mirrors the differential impact of alternative curriculum designs for primary education. The results of the analyses reveal that Chinese children acquire clock-reading knowledge—for analog and digital clocks—2 years earlier than their Flemish peers, due to a 2-year earlier exposure. Discussing these results, it is argued that the Flemish curriculum tends to underestimate what children are capable of and that clock-reading within the Flemish primary mathematics curriculum builds on dated assumptions and research results. Building on these outcomes, it is argued that curriculum development should be the result of collaborative inquiry involving policy makers, researchers, and teachers.

KEYWORDS

clock-reading knowledge comparative research mathematics curriculum textbook analysis 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Supplementary material

References

  1. Askey, R. (1999). Knowing and teaching elementary mathematics. American Educator, 23, 1–8.Google Scholar
  2. Basic Education Curriculum Material Development Centre (2001). National mathematics curriculum standards at the compulsory education level. Beijing, China: Beijing Normal University.Google Scholar
  3. Boulton-Lewis, G., Wilss, L. & Mutch, S. (1997). Analysis of primary school children’s abilities and strategies for reading and recording time from analog and digital clocks. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 9, 136–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Burny, E., Valcke, M. & Desoete, A. (2009). Towards an agenda for studying learning and instruction focusing on time-related competences in children. Educational Studies, 35(5), 481–492. doi: 10.1080/03055690902879093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Burny, E., Valcke, M. & Desoete, A. (2012). Clock-reading: an underestimated topic in children with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 352–361. doi: 10.1177/0022219411407773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Case, R. (1992). The mind’s staircase. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Case, R., Sandieson, R. & Dennis, S. (1986). Two cognitive developmental approaches to the design of remedial instruction. Cognitive Development, 1, 293–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Friedman, W. J. & Laycock, F. (1989). Children’s analog and digital clock knowledge. Child Development, 60, 357–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Geary, D. C. (1996). International differences in mathematical achievement: Their nature, causes, and consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 133–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Griffin, S., Case, R. & Sandieson, R. (1992). Synchrony and asynchrony in the acquisition of children’s everyday mathematical knowledge. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Grouws, D. A., Smith, M. S. & Sztajn, P. (2004). The preparation and teaching of U.S. mathematics teachers: Grades 4 and 8. In P. Kloosterman & F. Lester (Eds.), The 1990 through 2000 mathematics assessment of the national assessment of educational progress: Results and interpretations (pp. 221–269). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.Google Scholar
  12. Hatano, G. & Inagaki, K. (1998). Cultural contexts of schooling revisited: A review of “The Learning Gap” from a cultural psychology perspective. In S. G. Paris & H. M. Wellman (Eds.), Global prospects for education: Development, culture and schooling. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  13. Jiang, Z. (1995). A brief comparison of the U.S. and Chinese middle school mathematics programs. School Science and Mathematics, 95, 187–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Johansson, M. (2005). Mathematics textbook: The link between the intended and the implemented curriculum? Paper presented at the The Mathematics Education into the 21st Century Project, Malaysia.Google Scholar
  15. Lee, S. Y. (1998). Mathematics learning and teaching in the school context: Reflections from cross-cultural comparisons. In S. G. Paris & H. M. Wellman (Eds.), Global prospects for education: Development, culture, and schooling. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  16. Lemaire, P. & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: Contributions to children’s learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 83–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Levin, I. & Gilat, I. (1983). A developmental analysis of early time concepts: The equivalence and additivity of the effect of interfering cues on duration comparisons of young children. Child Development, 54, 78–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ma, L. (1999). Knowing and teaching elementary mathematics. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Miller, K. F., Kelly, M. K. & Zhou, X. (2005). Learning mathematics in China and the United States: Cross-cultural insights in the nature and course of mathematical development. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  20. Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J. J. & Zhang, H. C. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of number-naming systems. Psychological Science, 6, 56–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Moy, R. & Peverly, S. T. (2005). Perceptions of mathematics curricula and teaching in China. Psychology in the Schools, 42, 251–258. doi: 10.1002/Pits.20076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O. & Foy, P. (2005). TIMSS 2003 international report on achievement in the mathematical cognitive domains: Findings from a developmental project. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center.Google Scholar
  23. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.Google Scholar
  24. Peverly, S. T. (2005). Moving past cultural homogeneity: Suggestions for comparisons of students’ educational outcomes in the United States and China. Psychology in the Schools, 42, 241–249. doi: 10.1002/Pits.20075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Philips, D. (2011). Aspects of educational transfer. International Handbook of Comparative Education, 22, 1061–1077.Google Scholar
  26. Piaget, J. (1969). The child’s conception of time. New York: Ballentine.Google Scholar
  27. Pingel, F. (2009). UNESCO guidebook on textbook research and textbook revision (2 revised and updatedth ed.). Paris, France: George Eckert Institute for International Textbook Research.Google Scholar
  28. Reys, R., Reys, B., Lapan, R., Holliday, G. & Wasman, D. (2003). Assessing the impact of standards-based middle grades mathematics curriculum materials on student achievement. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 34, 74–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Schmidt, W. H., McKnight, C. C., Houang, R. T., Wang, H. C., Wiley, D. E., Cogan, L. S. & Wolfe, R. G. (2001). Why schools matter: A cross-national comparison of curriculum and learning. San Francisco, CA: OSSEY-BASS.Google Scholar
  30. Siegler, R. S. (1988). Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 258–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Siegler, R. S. & McGilly, K. (1989). Strategy choices in children’s time-telling. In I. L. D. Zakay (Ed.), Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective. Oxford, UK: North Holland.Google Scholar
  32. Siegler, R. S. & Richards, D. D. (1979). Development of time, speed, and distance concepts. Developmental Psychology, 15, 288–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Siegler, R. S. & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: How do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229–293). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  34. Stevenson, H. W., Chen, C. S. & Lee, S. Y. (1993). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children—10 years later. Science, 259, 53–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stevenson, H. W. & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap: Why our schools are failing and what we can learn from Japanese and Chinese education. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
  36. Stigler, J. W., Lee, S. Y., Lucker, G. W. & Stevenson, H. W. (1982). Curriculum and achievement in mathematics: A study of elementary school children in Japan, Taiwan and the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 315–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Van Steenbrugge, H., Valcke, M. & Desoete, A. (2010). Mathematics learning difficulties in primary education: Teachers’ professional knowledge and the use of commercially available learning packages. Educational Studies, 36, CP3-71. doi: 10.1080/03055690903148639.Google Scholar
  38. Vlaams Verbond voor het Katholiek Onderwijs (1998). Mathematics curriculum standards [Wiskunde Leerplan]. Brussels, Belgium: VVKBaO.Google Scholar
  39. Wang, J. & Lin, E. (2009). A meta-analysis of comparative studies on Chinese and US students’ mathematics performance: Implications for mathematics education reform and research. Educational Research Review, 4, 177–195.Google Scholar
  40. Zhao, N. N., Valcke, M., Desoete, A., Verhaeghe, J. & Xu, K. (2011). Multilevel analysis on predicting mathematics performance in Chinese primary schools: Implications for practice. Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, 20, 503–520.Google Scholar
  41. Zhou, Z., Peverly, S. T., Boehm, A. E. & Chongde, L. (2000). American and Chinese children’s understanding of distance, time, and speed interrelations. Cognitive Development, 15, 215–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Zhou, Z., Peverly, S. T. & Lin, C. D. (2004). Cross- and within-cultural variations in children’s understanding of distance, time, and speed interrelationships: A follow-up study. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 165, 5–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© National Science Council, Taiwan 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elise Burny
    • 1
    Email author
  • Martin Valcke
    • 2
  • Annemie Desoete
    • 3
  • Johannes E. Hans Van Luit
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of Educational StudiesGhent UniversityGhentBelgium
  2. 2.Department of Educational StudiesGhent UniversityGhentBelgium
  3. 3.Department of Experimental, Clinical and Health PsychologyGhent UniversityGhentBelgium
  4. 4.Department of Social and Behavioral SciencesUtrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations