The smartphone has become an integral part of society, including our educational and professional lives. Smartphone use is highest amongst people aged 18–29, and therefore is highly represented in the University setting. Statistics show that 97% of students own a smartphone (Pew Research Center, 2021), and 95% of students bring that smartphone to class (Tindell & Bohlander, 2012). Given the frequency of smartphone use in the college student population, it is not surprising ample research has investigated smartphone use in the University setting – both inside and outside the classroom. There is a large body of literature that looks at smartphone use and academic performance. Smartphone is associated with lower GPA’s, both in self-reports (Ibrahim et al., 2018; Katz & Lambert, 2016; Kim et al., 2019), and actual GPA (Hawi & Samaha, 2017). Research has also found that smartphone use is associated with poor sleep quantity (Demirci et al., 2015), life satisfaction (Lachmann et al., 2018), and anxiety, loneliness, and depression (Boumosleh & Jaalouk, 2017) in college students.

Within the classroom, smartphones can often be a cause of distraction, as students use the phone during class to check social media (Gupta & Irwin, 2016), multi-task (Sana et al., 2013), or contact friends (Tindell & Bohlander, 2012). These activities deflect from instruction, and impede student learning. Although a substantial body of research has found that cell phone use in the classroom is associated with lower academic achievement (e.g. Amez & Baert, 2020), fewer studies have examined the effects of cell phone use on students’ psychological well-being in the classroom setting, and even fewer have examined the impact of cell phone use using a quasi-experimental paradigm. The present study aims to further explore the impact that the smartphone has on course comprehension, and expand the research by investigating how smartphone use in the classroom impacts the psychological well-being of students during class.

Research on smartphones in the classroom is mixed, and primarily focuses on academic performance. On one hand, when used properly, smartphones are associated with better academic performance. The convenience of the smartphone allows students to access the internet anywhere, letting them connect with information, assignments, and e-mails related to school almost instantly (Lepp et al., 2014). Also, social networking sites and online applications contribute to easy communication amongst students and the professor, which allows for seamless collaboration (Chen & Ji, 2015). Some research has found that the more students engage in course-related activities on their phone, the more likely they are to seek out additional information to comprehend the material (Rashid & Asghar, 2016).

On the other hand, the smartphone is often a distraction for students, which takes away from the classroom experience and retention of information. The smartphone can serve as a source of entertainment for students, rather than a working instrument. It has been found to draw students’ attention away from study time and time spent on homework and assignments (Junco & Cotton, 2012), ultimately taking away from the learning experience. Given this information, it is not surprising that smartphone use is associated with lower self-reported (Ibrahim et al., 2018; Katz & Lambert, 2016; Kim et al., 2019), and actual GPA (Hawi & Samaha, 2017; see Amez and Baert (2020) for full literature review).

There is ample research on the smartphones’ impact on academic performance, but less is known about its potential impact on course-related psychological well-being. Anxiety is particularly problematic among college students, as it often impedes the learning experience (Mazzone et al., 2007). 60.8% of college students report feeling overwhelming amounts of anxiety last year alone (American College Health Association, 2022).

In general, research has found a negative correlation between smartphone use and psychological well-being, specifically in anxiety and depression (e.g. Demirci et al., 2012). While the majority of this research have examined smartphone use and overall levels psychological well-being, some studies have examined this relationship in the classroom setting. Again, the literature has found a negative relationship (e.g. Boumosleh & Jaalouk, 2017) Two, not mutually exclusive, explanations for this relationship have been suggested. One, the barrage of alerts on our phone and constant streams of information creates feelings of anxiousness, and distraction from the lecture (Al-Furaih & Al-Awidi, 2021). Two, smartphones in the class can create anxiety due to FOMO, or fear of missing out (e.g. Yildirim & Correia, 2015), as students notice other things going on amongst friends while they are in-class. Given these distractions, removing the smartphone from the classroom experience will likely reduce student anxiety (Stankovic et al., 2021).

Research has also found that mindfulness – defined as the quality or state of being conscious or aware of present surroundings—can significantly reduce anxiety (Hoffman et al., 2010). This is particularly true in the classroom. Mindfulness during lectures has been found to be associated with better grades (Caballero et al., 2019), and better overall psychological health while learning (Mahfouz et al., 2018). With the distraction of the smartphone, it is likely that smartphone use reduces students’ mindfulness during lectures, inhibiting the learning experience and increasing anxiety.

Due to the conflicting research on the effects of smartphone use in the classroom, the current study seeks to clarify and expand the impact of academic achievement by investigating the effects of smartphone use on course comprehension using a quasi-experimental procedure. In addition, we hope to explore the effects of smartphone use on psychological well-being – operationally defined as classroom anxiety and mindfulness. In the present study, students were assigned to either a quasi-experimental group, where students were instructed to leave their smartphone on the windowsill / desk of the instructor as they entered the class – ensuring a physical distance from their phone – or a control group, where they received no instructions on smartphone use. Students then completed a survey to measure levels of course comprehension and psychological well-being based on these conditions. The present study tested three hypotheses:

  • H1: Based on previous research, students who were physically distanced from their cell phones would be less distracted, and therefore have significantly higher rates of course comprehension then students in the control group.

  • H2: Students who were physically distanced from their cell phones would be less distracted and more engaged in the lecture, therefore having lower levels of anxiety then students in the control group.

  • H3: Students who were physically distanced from their cell phones would be less distracted, and therefore have significantly higher rates of mindfulness then students in the control group.

Methods

Participants

Participants were undergraduate students at the [BLINDED], a primarily undergraduate institution in New York City. Four content courses in the Behavioral Sciences Department were selected for participation in the study in Spring 2020. The courses included were Introduction to Sociology (n = 54), Dynamics of Violence (n = 18), Educational Psychology (n = 21), and Health Psychology (n = 15). There were 36 participants (33%) in the quasi-experimental group, and 72 in the control group (67%).

The participants (N = 108) included 59 females (55%) and 49 males (45%). Of this total, 44 were Asian American (41%), 32 identified as White / Caucasian (30%), 11 identified as Latinx or Hispanic (10%), 11 identified as Black / African American (10%), and 10 were unidentified (10%). Students ranged in age from 18 to 47 (M = 20.2, SD = 3.6). Ethical approval was granted by [BLINDED] Review Board (Protocol number: ESB 1520).

Procedure

Of the four behavioral science courses, two courses – Introductory Sociology and Dynamics of Violence – were treated as controls. Students in these courses did not receive any instructions or specific restrictions on their smartphone use. Educational Psychology and Health Psychology were assigned as the quasi-experimental condition. At the beginning of class each day, students were instructed to place their phones on a desk at the front of the classroom before the lecture was given. The phones could not be physically on them or accessed throughout the duration of the course. In the beginning of March 2020 – after six weeks of in-person participation in the course—all students completed a self-report survey that measured their course comprehension, mindfulness, and anxiety throughout the course.

Instruments

Course comprehension

A 10-item questionnaire was created, which specifically assessed how engaged the student felt in the course material during the course. Sample items included, “I feel confident in my knowledge of the course material,” or “It is clear to me what concepts I do not understand after the lecture.” Questions ranged on a scale from “1” – strongly disagree – to “5” – strongly agree. The items were averaged and reliability was very good (α = 0.86). A reliability analysis was conducted, and was best when all 10 items were included in the scale. In addition, a principal component analysis was conducted to investigate construct validity. All ten items had eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and the cumulative variance explained was 50.08%. See Appendix 1 for questions.

Anxiety

A 7-item questionnaire was created, which specifically assessed students’ anxiety during class. Sample items included “During class, I feel nervous, anxious, or on edge” or “During class, I have trouble relaxing.” Questions ranged on a scale from “1” – not at all – to “5” – all the time. The items were averaged and reliability was very good. (α = 0.93). A reliability analysis was conducted, and was best when all 7 items were included in the scale. In addition, a principal component analysis was conducted to investigate construct validity. All seven items had eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and the cumulative variance explained by the 7 items was 71.49%. See Appendix 2 for questions.

Mindfulness

A 10-item questionnaire was created which specifically assessed students’ mindfulness during their respective course. Sample items included, “I take notes on autopilot, without truly processing the information” and “I am focused on outside responsibilities or tasks during class.” Questions ranged on a scale from “1” – strongly disagree – to “5” – strongly agree. The items were averaged and reliability was very good. (α = 0.94). A reliability analysis was conducted, and was best when all 10 items were included in the scale. In addition, a principal component analysis was conducted to investigate construct validity. All ten items had eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and the cumulative variance explained was 64.49%. See Appendix 3 for questions. See Table 1 for correlations between study variables.

Table 1 Bivariate correlations, means, & standard deviations

Results

Smartphone use and course comprehension

An independent sample t-test examined the effect that smartphone use in the classroom had on overall course comprehension. Results indicated statistically significant differences in course comprehension, t(106) = -2.55, p = 0.01, d = 0.56. The quasi-experimental group had significantly higher levels of course comprehension (M = 4.16, SD = 0.56) than the control group (M = 3.86, SD = 0.56).

Smartphone use and anxiety

An independent sample t-test examined the effect that smartphone use in the classroom had on anxiety during the course. Results indicated statistically significant differences in course anxiety, t(106) = 2.27, p = 0.03, d = 0.88. The experimental group had significantly lower levels of anxiety (M = 1.48, SD = 0.67) than the control group (M = 1.84, SD = 0.97). See Fig. 1 for results.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The effects of smartphone use on course comprehension and psychological well-being in the classroom

Smartphone use and mindfulness

An independent sample t-test examined the effect that smartphone use in the classroom had on overall mindfulness during class. Results indicated statistically significant differences in mindfulness, t(106) = -2.84, p = 0.01, d = 0.84. The experimental group had significantly higher levels of mindfulness (M = 4.19, SD = 0.73) than the control group (M = 3.71, SD = 0.89). See Fig. 1 for mean comparisons between study variables.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to explore whether smartphone use impacts course comprehension and the psychological state of students in the classroom. Results found that students who physically removed their smartphones from them throughout the duration of the class had significantly higher levels of comprehension and mindfulness in the course. In addition to that, students without their smartphones had significantly lower levels of anxiety. The data provides preliminary evidence that limiting cell phone use creates a more positive psychological state for students, and in turn, may yield more positive learning outcomes (e.g. Bóo et al., 2020).

The negative association between cell phone use and course comprehension is consistent with previous studies (see e.g. De Shields & Riley, 2019; Kuznekoff & Titsworth, 2013). This finding adds to the growing body of literature that suggests that distracted students perform worse in the classroom. However, previous research has largely been correlational, leaving open the possibility of alternative explanations. The present study provides quasi-experimental evidence that smartphone use has a causal and negative influence on classroom experience. Additionally, the present study adds to the literature that cell phone use increases anxiety in the classroom and reduces mindfulness. This finding is consistent with Lepp et al. (2014), that also identified a negative association between cell phone use and college students’ general level of anxiety. These findings provide preliminary evidence that the presence of cell phones in the class negatively affects student perceptions of their classroom experience.

It should be noted that the results from this study are prior to the COVID-19 outbreak, which may change the nature of psychological well-being in the classroom. Anxiety in the classroom prior to the COVID-19 pandemic focused on school work and grades, whereas post-pandemic may focus on transmission of diseases, vaccinations, and social anxiety due to lack of exposure to social settings. Future studies may investigate psychological well-being of students in the classroom post the COVID-19 pandemic.

Limitations

The conclusions we can draw are limited by the design of the study and the measures we used. With respect to study design, the participants came from four distinct classes, and different instructors taught each of those classes and the number of participants in each group were not equal. Although each class was a content class in the Behavioral Science Department, future studies may investigate the effects of smartphones in the classroom using the same classes or a single instructor in order to avoid potential confounds. Additionally, our design only examined whether smartphones affect the classroom experience across students. Future studies can include a baseline assessment of the outcomes at the beginning of the semester and again at the end to examine whether smart phones in the classroom have a within-student effect.

The outcomes of the study relied on self-report data from non-validated questionnaires. Self-report data can be biased and future studies should include objective measures of course comprehension (e.g., grade in class) and psychological well-being (e.g., current use of antidepressant drugs). While self-report measures have limitations, the bias associated with these measures were evenly distributed across conditions, therefore the effect of condition cannot be explained by reliance on self-report data. The use of non-validated questionnaires also limits the validity of the results. Future studies can use the items from the present study and items from validity measures to ensure high convergent validity. While many valid and reliable measures of anxiety and mindfulness exist, none perfectly fit the aim of the present study— to examine these constructs confined to the experience in one specific classroom. Thus, questionnaires were created to fit the study design. Only items with face validity were included, and each item was meant to examine the larger construct directly and clearly. Additionally, the high reliability scores suggests that these items were testing a single construct.

Conclusion

The results from our study provide evidence that the use of smartphones in the classroom has a negative effect on levels of course comprehension and the psychological state of students during lecture. Given the psychological state of students is imperative to creating a positive learning environment (Febrilia et al., 2011), it is important that educators make informed decisions about technology use in the classroom, in order to maintain a high-quality learning experience. Something as simple as limiting smartphone use during scheduled class time can have an impact on the well-being of students, and in turn, create a better learning environment.