Skip to main content
Log in

Can you spare some change for charity? Experimental evidence on verbal cues and loose change effects in a Dictator Game

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We use a simple laboratory experiment to measure the effect on altruism of (i) whether the participants’ choices are presented verbally or non-verbally, and (ii) whether the participants have a large amount of loose change. We find strong evidence for the first effect and weaker evidence for the second. These effects may explain some of the variation in the average level of generosity found in different Dictator Game results.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. However, it is possible that in such surveys people overestimate how much money they have given to charity. The average donation in Yörük’s data is higher than the average in data from other sources.

  2. As noted by Meer and Rosen (2011), there is a possible endogeneity problem here too: universities with more resources may be able to approach more alumni.

  3. There are two existing studies of charitable giving that compare laboratory and field results, although they do not investigate communication effects directly: Benz and Meier (2008) and Carlsson et al. (2013). Benz and Meier find that university students are more likely to give to a certain set of charities when invited to do so in a Dictator Game than when they are invited to do so while paying their tuition fees each semester. Here the laboratory versus field effect may be confounded by the fact that the Dictator Game earnings are a windfall gain, so Carlsson et al. use a 2 × 2 design to test for both the windfall effect and the laboratory versus field effect. They find that generosity is significantly higher when the income is a windfall rather than earned, but in addition, generosity is significantly higher in the laboratory than in the field.

  4. Some experimental results on charitable giving are suggestive of a loose change effect. For example, in Brown et al. (2013), which is about whether people prefer to give time or money, participants earn an amount which depends on how well they complete a manual task. In one treatment they cannot donate any money to charity until they have completed the task, and in this case many participants choose to donate coins but keep notes for themselves. In Etang et al. (2012), participants are paid $20 made up of a $10 note, $5 note, two $2 coins and a $1 coin, and then invited to donate some or all of the payment to charity. Among participants choosing to donate anything the modal donation is $5, and the vast majority of those giving $5 use all of their coins.

  5. Of the three field experiments, the one most relevant to our study involves drivers at a gas station being paid either five $1 notes, five 1$ coins, or one $5 note. In principle these treatments could be used to distinguish a coins-versus-notes effect from a denomination effect. However, as the authors note, the coin treatment is confounded by the fact that such coins are uncommon in the US and might be valuable as souvenirs. In fact, spending is lowest in the coin treatment.

  6. In New Zealand, the smallest denomination note is the $5 note. At the time of the experiment, one New Zealand dollar was worth about 0.78 US dollars.

  7. All sessions were run on the same day. In the morning we ran two NLC and two NMC sessions; in the afternoon we ran two VLC and two VMC sessions. A coin toss determined which sessions were less change and which were more change treatments. Participants were asked to indicate which sessions they were available for, and were randomly allocated to a session subject to their availability.

  8. However, one of these five participants gave the whole $19: possibly this participant was an extreme altruist who would have given all of his/her payment in any treatment.

  9. The average donation across the verbal treatments represents 12.5 % of a participant’s endowment. This is lower than in some other Dictator Games in which the recipient is a charity, but is very similar to the Reinstein and Riener (2012) “performance/cash” treatment. As in our verbal treatment, their performance/cash treatment has a strict double-blind protocol, participants are not paid a show-up fee, they have to earn their endowment, and they are paid before making a donation decision.

  10. In results reported in an earlier version of the paper (Fielding and Knowles 2013), the verbal treatments were constructed in a slightly different way from the ones described in the main text, but included the same number of participants. In the earlier verbal treatments, participants heard the verbal solicitation while they were together in Room B, and then went one at a time into Room A to put their donation (if any) into an envelope, and the envelope into the red box. Total donations in these verbal treatments were almost identical to the ones reported in the main text, and there was a large verbal treatment effect on both the number of donations and the amount donated (p < 0.01). But in addition, within the verbal treatments the Mann–Whitney test indicated a significant loose change effect on the amount donated (p < 0.01). Pooling the two earlier verbal treatments with the two verbal treatments in the main text also produces a significant Mann–Whitney statistic for the loose change effect (p = 0.02). We suspect that there is some loose change effect at work, but this may not always be apparent unless the sample is very large.

  11. Of course this does not invalidate the inferences made from inter-treatment differences in existing laboratory experiments.

References

  • Andreoni, J., & Rao, J. M. (2011). The power of asking: how communication affects selfishness, empathy, and altruism. Journal of Public Economics, 95(7–8), 513–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., Rao, J. M., & Trachtman, H. (2011). Avoiding the ask: A field experiment on altruism, empathy, and charitable giving. New York: Mimeo.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Benz, M., & Meier, S. (2008). Do people behave in experiments as in the field? Evidence from donations. Experimental Economics, 11(3), 268–281.

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., Meer, J., & Williams, J. F. (2013). Why do people volunteer? An experimental analysis of preferences for time donations. NBER working paper 19066.

  • Carlsson, F., He, H., & Martinsson, P. (2013). Easy come, easy go. The role of windfall money in lab and field experiments. Experimental Economics, 16, 190–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dana, J., Cain, D. M., & Dawes, R. M. (2006). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Costly (but quiet) exit in dictator games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100, 193–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dana, J., Weber, R. A., & Kuang, J. X. (2007). Exploiting moral wiggle room: experiments demonstrating an illusory preference for fairness. Economic Theory, 33, 67–80.

    Article  MATH  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, W. D., & Noble, S. M. (2001). Defensive response to charitable direct mail solicitations. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 15(3), 2–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etang, A., Fielding, D., & Knowles, S. (2012). Giving to Africa and perceptions of poverty. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(4), 819–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fielding, D., & Knowles, S. (2013). Can you spare some change for charity? Experimental evidence on verbal cues and loose change effects in a dictator game. University of Otago Department of Economics Working Paper 1318.

  • Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16(18), 1824–1829.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, M. A., & Johnson, E. L. (2006). Solicitation and donation: An econometric evaluation of alumni generosity in higher education. International Journal of Educational Advancement, 8(4), 268–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, C., & Soll, J. (1996). Mental budgeting and consumer decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 23(1), 40–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltá, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297(5582), 846–848.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, C. E., Lange, A., List, J. A., Price, M. K., & Rupp, N. G. (2006). Toward an understanding of the economics of charity: evidence from a field experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(2), 747–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, L. L., & Ramey, G. (1988). Donor behaviour and voluntary support for higher education institutions. Journal of Higher Education, 59(2), 115–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • List, J., Berrens, R., Bohara, A., & Kerkvilet, J. (2004). Examining the role of social isolation on stated preferences. American Economic Review, 94, 741–752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meer, J. (2011). Brother, can you spare a dime? Peer pressure in charitable solicitation. Journal of Public Economics, 95(7–8), 926–941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meer, J., & Rosen, H. S. (2011). The ABCs of charitable solicitation. Journal of Public Economics, 95(5–6), 363–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, H., Mishra, A., & Nayakankuppam, D. (2006). A bias for the whole. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(4), 541–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raghubir, P., & Srivastava, J. (2009). The denomination effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(4), 701–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinstein, D., & Riener, G. (2012). Decomposing desert and tangibility effects in a charitable giving experiment. Experimental Economics, 15, 229–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schervish, P. G., & Havens, J. J. (1997). Social participation and charitable giving A multivariate analysis. VOLUNTAS: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, 8(3), 235–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Boston: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tonin, M., & Vlassopoulos, M. (2013). Sharing one’s fortune? An experimental study on earned income and giving, IZA Discussion Paper No. 7294.

  • Van Diepen, M., Donkers, B., & Franses, P. H. (2009a). Does irritation induced by charitable direct mailings reduce donations? International Journal of Marketing Research, 26(3), 180–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Diepen, M., Donkers, B., & Franses, P. H. (2009b). Dynamic and competitive effects of direct mailings: a charitable giving approach. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(1), 120–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandoros, S. (2013). My five pounds are not as good as yours, so I will spend them. Experimental Economics, 16(4), 546–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yörük, B. K. (2009). How responsive are charitable donors to requests to give? Journal of Public Economics, 93(9–10), 1111–1117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Reece Pomeroy, Robbie Bell and Andrew Tan for their invaluable work as research assistants on this project, and to Phillip Hall and Maroš Servátka for helpful suggestions on our experimental design. An earlier version of the paper was presented at the Asia Pacific Economic Science Association Conference (Auckland, February 2014) and the University of Otago Seminar Programme (Dunedin, May 2014); we are grateful to participants for the useful comments received. We are also grateful to two anonymous referees and the editor for helpful comments on an earlier draft.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Fielding.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 19 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fielding, D., Knowles, S. Can you spare some change for charity? Experimental evidence on verbal cues and loose change effects in a Dictator Game. Exp Econ 18, 718–730 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-014-9424-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-014-9424-x

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation