1 Introduction

Many students have serious cognitive problems with algebra, in particular with seeing structure and making sense of algebraic formulas with their abstract symbols (Arcavi, Drijvers, & Stacey, 2017; Drijvers, Goddijn, & Kindt, 2011; Kieran, 2006). The teaching of algebra often focuses on basic skills through practicing algebraic calculation in similar tasks (Arcavi et al., 2017). However, many students experience problems with when to use these basic skills and finding strategies to solve algebra problems: they lack symbol sense (Arcavi et al., 2017; Hoch & Dreyfus, 2005, 2010; Oehrtman, Carlson, & Thompson, 2008; Thompson, 2013). Symbol sense concerns a very general notion of “when and how” to use symbols (Arcavi, 1994), and it functions as a compass when using basic skills (Drijvers et al., 2011). A lack of symbol sense leads to an overreliance on basic skills, just learned methods, and on the symbolic representations, leading to poor achievements (Kieran, 2006; Knuth, 2000; Eisenberg & Dreyfus, 1994; Pierce & Stacey, 2007). However, it is not clear how to teach symbol sense appropriately (Arcavi, 2005; Hoch & Dreyfus, 2005). In a previous study, we showed how teaching graphing formulas by hand, defined as graphing using recognition and reasoning, without technology, to grade 11 students improved their insight into algebraic formulas (Kop, Janssen, Drijvers, & Van Driel, 2020). Insight into algebraic formulas is an aspect of symbol sense and involves recognizing structure and key features of a formula and qualitative reasoning with and about a formula. In the study presented here, we investigated whether graphing formulas by hand abilities is related to students’ symbol sense in a broader sense, that is, symbol sense while solving non-routine algebra tasks. Doing so, the study aims to contribute to our theoretical knowledge of students’ symbol sense abilities and to inform teaching practice.

2 Theoretical background

The most important theoretical notion that guides this study is symbol sense. Fey (1990) was the first to mention symbol sense and described it as an informal skill required to deal effectively with symbolic expressions and algebraic operations. According to Fey, goals for teaching symbol sense would include at least the following basic themes: the ability to scan an algebraic expression to make rough estimates of the patterns that would emerge in numerical or graphical representation, to make comparisons of orders of magnitude for functions, and to inspect algebraic operations and predict the form of the result and judge the likelihood that it has been performed correctly. Arcavi (1994) elaborated on this idea and broadened the concept to all phases in the problem-solving cycle (Pierce & Stacey, 2004). According to Arcavi (ibid.), symbol sense would include:

  • An understanding and a feel for the power of symbols, that is, understanding how and when symbols can be used to display relationships, generalizations, and proofs and when to abandon symbols in favor of other approaches in order to make progress with a problem

  • An ability to manipulate and to “read” symbolic expressions as two complementary aspects of solving algebra problems

  • The awareness that one can successfully engineer symbolic relationships which express the verbal or graphical information needed to make progress in a problem, and the ability to engineer those expressions

Drijvers et al. (2011) described symbol sense in relation to basic skills: symbol sense and basic skills are complementary. Basic skills involve procedural work with a local focus and an emphasis on algebraic calculations, whereas symbol sense involves strategic work, taking a global view on algebraic expressions/formulas and algebraic reasoning. A global view, or a Gestalt view, has to do with the ability to see an algebraic formula as a whole, to “read through” it, and to recognize its structure and global characteristics.

Related to the notion of symbol sense, Pierce and Stacey (2004) used the term algebraic insight to capture the symbol sense in transformational activities in the “solving” phase of the problem-solving cycle (from mathematical problem to mathematical solution) when using Computer Algebra Systems (CAS). Algebraic insight has to do with the recognition and identification of structure, objects, key features, dominant terms, and meanings of symbols and the ability to link representations (Kenney, 2008; Pierce & Stacey, 2004). Kenney (2008) used this framework of Pierce and Stacey for her research and added “know how and when to use symbols” and “know when to abandon a representation.”

To develop symbol sense, graphing formulas by hand might be useful because it involves many aspects of symbol sense. First, graphing formulas is about linking the symbolic and graphical representation. Second, to efficiently graph formulas by hand, one has to recognize the structure and key features of a formula and to reason with and about formulas. Third, graphing a formula can be considered a visualization of a formula, and using such a visualization in problem solving requires knowing about what is represented and where to look for it. We will now elaborate on these aspects.

Linking representations, such as formulas and graphs, is important in learning about functions (Janvier, 1987; Leinhardt, Zavlavsky, & Stein, 1990) and might be used to give meaning to algebraic formulas (Kieran, 2006). Duval (1999) used the term registers of representations to indicate that each representation (formula or graph) has its own specific means and processing for mathematical thinking. He distinguished two types of transformation: treatments, transformations in the same representation, and conversions, transformations from one representation to another, like from formulas into Cartesian graphs. Conversions are at the core of understanding mathematics, but many students have problems learning these conversions, as it requires a change of register and the recognition of the same represented object in different representations (Duval, 1999, 2006).

In efficiently graphing formulas by hand, many aspects of symbol sense are involved. Research in expertise in graphing formulas by hand shows that experts’ strategies could be described with combinations of different levels of recognition and qualitative reasoning (Kop, Janssen, Drijvers, Veenman, & Van Driel, 2015). For recognition, experts use a repertoire of function families with their characteristics and key graph features like zeroes and turning points. In qualitative reasoning, the focus is on the global shape of the graph, ignoring what is not relevant in the situation, and using global descriptions. Qualitative reasoning is often used by experts in complex problem situations that are difficult to look through in detail, like in physical models (Bredeweg & Forbus, 2003). In the domain of graphing formulas, experts tend to use qualitative reasoning to explore (parts of) the graph, for instance, infinity behavior, increasing/decreasing of functions, stronger/weaker components of a function, and in the composition of two subgraphs, after decomposing a formula in two sub-functions. Graphing formulas by hand is related to covariational reasoning, which is about coordinating two covarying quantities while attending to how they change in relation to each other (Thompson, 2013; Carlson, Jacobs, Coe, Larsen, & Hsu, 2002). This covariational reasoning is critical in supporting student learning of functions in secondary and undergraduate mathematics (Carlson et al., 2002; Oehrtman et al., 2008). Carlson, Madison, and West (2015) showed that students were not able to reason “as the value of x gets larger the value of y decreases, and as the value of x approaches 2, the value of y increases” when they had to link the formula of f(x) = 1/(x − 2)2 to its graph.

Visualizing formulas through graphs is used in problem solving for understanding the problem situation, recording information, exploring, and monitoring and evaluating results (Polya, 1945; Stylianou & Silver, 2004). Stylianou and Silver (2004) compared experts and novices in solving algebra problems and showed that experts know how to use graphs in solving algebra problems. Experts “see” relevant relations visualized in the graph and can use the graph for visual and qualitative explorations. Although novices have some declarative knowledge, they lack the necessary procedural knowledge to construct visual representations of general functions and to explore the graphs they have constructed. Such exploration requires a global view of the whole graph and not just a local apprehension (Duval, 1999) and is only possible when one is very familiar with the function (Stylianou & Silver, 2004). This matched Eisenberg and Dreyfus’ (1994) ideas about the need for a repertoire of basic functions that one should simultaneously “see” in a graph as one thinks of the algebraic formula.

In sum, graphing formulas involves many essential aspects of symbol sense to solve algebra problems, like visualizing a formula through a Cartesian graph, taking a global view to read through a formula and enable recognition of the structure of a formula and/or its key features, and qualitative reasoning. In the current study, therefore, we focus on the relation between symbol sense involved in graphing formulas by hand and the symbol sense to solve non-routine algebra tasks. Aspects of symbol sense, learned and used in the context of graphing formulas, might be used in a broader domain of algebra tasks. In this study, this broader domain is restricted to algebra tasks that can be solved with graphs and reasoning, so without the use of algebraic calculations.

2.1 Research questions

The theoretical perspective described in the previous section led to the following main research question:

How do grade 12 students’ abilities to graph formulas by hand relate to their use of symbol sense while solving non-routine algebra tasks?

We formulated two sub-questions. We expected a relation between students’ abilities to graph formulas by hand and their abilities to solve algebra tasks with symbol sense, because graphing formulas can be seen as a subset of algebra tasks, and in graphing formulas by hand, many aspects of symbol sense are involved. This led to the first sub-question:

To what extent are students’ graphing formulas by hand abilities positively correlated to their abilities to solve algebra tasks with symbol sense?

In graphing formulas by hand, several symbol sense aspects are involved, and we expected that students would be able to use these symbol sense aspects also in the context of solving algebra tasks. In addition, we expected that when one is able to graph formulas by hand, one would see more possibilities to use this strategy (making a graph). This led to the second sub-question:

How is students’ symbol sense use in graphing formulas similar or different from their symbol sense use in solving non-routine algebra tasks?

3 Method

We first describe the context of the study, that is, the position of this study in a larger research project; next, we address the participants, the symbol sense test, the data, and the way the data were analyzed.

3.1 Context of the study

This study is part of a larger PhD research project about studying how symbol sense might be taught. In two previous studies, we analyzed expertise in graphing formulas by hand and identified main components of symbol sense used by experts, that is, recognition of function families and key features from the structure of formulas and qualitative reasoning. In a third study, a group of 21 students from the first author’s school were taught how to graph formulas by hand, using recognition and reasoning, in a series of five lessons of 90 min. The series of lessons started with the recognition of basic function families with their characteristics. Students learned about transformations and about using qualitative reasoning by focusing on the global shape of the graphs and using global descriptions (e.g., “it is a root-graph reversed”). Then, these basic function families were used as building blocks when graphing more complex functions, like in y = 2x + 4/x and \(y=2x\sqrt{5-x}\). Complex functions could be decomposed in two basic functions, which could both be graphed. Explicit attention was paid to the composition of the two subgraphs through qualitative reasoning. In a subsequent task, the focus was on recognizing graph features, like zeroes and turning points. When recognition falls short, one can do strategic explorations of parts of the graphs. In a subsequent task, students learned how to use qualitative reasoning for determining, e.g., infinity behavior of a function and increasing/decreasing. In Appendix 3, more details are given about this intervention. Pre-test results of this third study showed that the students had a lot of trouble with graphing formulas by hand; post-test results showed an improvement of their abilities. The current study is the fourth study, which focused on the relation between symbol sense involved in graphing formulas by hand and in solving algebra tasks.

3.2 Instruments

The main instrument developed for this study was a test on students’ competencies and symbol sense use when graphing formulas by hand and when solving non-routine algebra tasks. Two types of tasks were used: type A tasks, in which the link between formula and graph was explicitly indicated, and type B tasks with no reference to graphs in the text. Students were asked to explain their answers.

The test was constructed in three steps. First, we looked for tasks that were used in other studies and adjusted them to our situation. Second, a first draft of the test was discussed with a professor in mathematics education and an experienced teacher. They were asked whether the tasks fit the grade 12 curriculum and whether they thought the students should be able to solve these tasks. Third, using their feedback, the test was constructed with eight type A tasks and twelve type B tasks. All teachers of the students involved in the study indicated that they thought that these tasks were challenging but, according to the curriculum, should be doable.

In the type A tasks, we explicitly used the word “graph” and addressed different aspects of linking formulas to graphs. Some of these kinds of tasks have been used more often in research: working with parameters (Drijvers et al., 2011; Heid, Thomas, & Zbiek, 2013), reverse thinking (finding a formula with a graph) (Keller, 1994; Drijvers et al., 2011; Duval, 2006), and evaluating a (part of the) graph made with a graphic calculator. The test started with type B tasks, because type A tasks might suggest using graphs in the type B tasks.

Type B tasks could be solved with only graphs and reasoning, but no explicit links to graphs were given in the text. These tasks should give information about the students’ symbol sense use while solving algebra tasks. Some tasks have been used by others in assessing students’ algebraic competences: number of solutions (Heid et al., 2013), inequalities (Kenney, 2008; Tsamir & Bazzini, 2004), and reasoning about the function (Kenney, 2008; Pierce & Stacey, 2007). In Appendix 1, we give the symbol sense test. The internal consistency and reliability of both types of tasks was deemed acceptable, based on Cronbach’s alpha on the type A tasks being 0.70 and on the type B tasks 0.72. Deleting any task hardly changed Cronbach’s alpha.

3.3 Participants

In this study, 114 grade 12 students from six different schools throughout the Netherlands were involved. The students had 45 min to finish the pen-and-paper test. All students were enrolled in the Dutch mathematics B course that prepares for university studies in mathematics, physics, and engineering. In regular education in the Netherlands, students learn about linear, quadratic, and exponential functions in grade 8 and 9. In grade 10, the graphic calculator is introduced, and power, rational, logarithmic functions are studied. In grade 11 and 12, calculus topics such as derivatives and integrals are taught. Graphing formulas by hand, without technology, is not a specific subject in the Dutch curriculum: graphs are normally made with the graphic calculator. Therefore, we expected that many students would have difficulties to graph formulas by hand and that they would score low on the graphing tasks in the symbol sense test. To investigate the relation between graphing formulas abilities and the abilities to solve algebra tasks, a broad range of scores on the graphing tasks was needed. To ensure this range of graphing abilities and to investigate how the teaching of graphing formulas by hand would affect students' symbol sense abilities, 21 students from the third study (who were taught how to graph formulas by hand) were involved in the current study. The teachers of the five schools that were involved in the study volunteered to participate, and differed with respect to years of teaching experience.

3.4 Procedure

In February 2017, the symbol sense test was administered to the 114 students. For each student, all answers on the tasks were scored as correct (score = 1), partly correct (score between 0 and 1), or incorrect (score = 0). For each student, the sum of the scores on the type A tasks resulted in a TA score and the sum on type B tasks in a TB score. In addition, for both type A and type B tasks, the students’ strategies were encoded, as far as these could be recognized from the written material. We looked for symbol sense strategies, like recognition of key features, decomposition in sub-formulas, and reasoning and for other strategies, like making calculations (derivatives, and/or points). In the type B tasks, we also registered whether making a graph or a relevant part of the graph was used, as these were considered symbol sense strategies. When, in these type B tasks, symbol sense strategies were used, a strategy score of 1 was given. However, when calculations were made, the strategy score was 0. The sum of these scores resulted in a StratTB score for each student. Besides the StratTB score, an effective strategy score (EffStratTB score) was also calculated, because using a symbol sense strategy did not guarantee a correct solution. When the symbol sense strategy resulted in a score of 0.5 or higher on a task, the effective strategy score was 1. The sum resulted in an EffStratTB score for each student.

The scores on the written test, TA, TB, StratTB, and EffStratTB scores, are considered to be related to the general mathematics ability of the students, and the general mathematics ability of each of the 114 students was rated by their own teacher on a scale from 1 to 10 (called Math rating). In a one-way independent Anova on the students’ Math ratings, no significant differences between the six schools were found.

In addition to the strategy scores from the written tests, we wanted a more detailed picture of the relation between the symbol sense use in the graphing tasks and in the algebra tasks (sub-question 2). As we expected that symbol sense involved in graphing formulas might be used in solving algebra tasks, we asked six students who belonged to the group of 25% highest scoring students on the graphing tasks (scores of 3 to 7.5 out of max 8) to think aloud during the test. Two of these students had very high Math ratings (T and K), two had more than average Math ratings (A and M), and two had average Math ratings (Y and I). As our aim is to teach symbol sense to all students, these six students were also involved in the teaching of graphing formulas by hand. Thinking aloud is not expected to disturb thinking processes and should give reliable information about problem-solving activities (Ericsson, 2006). The thinking-aloud protocols were transcribed.

3.5 Data analysis

The first sub-question was about the relation between the TA scores and the three scores on type B tasks (TB scores, StratTB scores, EffStratTB scores). The assumptions of regression, independent errors, homoscedasticity, normally distributed errors, and multicollinearity were met (Field, 2012). Because of the small number of items, the scores on the type B tasks were not normally distributed; therefore, bias corrected and accelerated bootstrap 95% CIs are reported.

The Math ratings were related to the scores on the type B tasks. To explore the relation between TA scores and scores on type B tasks, we first used regression with the type B scores as dependent variables and the TA scores as independent variable. Then, the Math rating was added also as an independent variable, to explore the influence of the Math rating on the scores of the type B tasks.

To get a more detailed picture of the relation between the TA scores and the scores on the type B tasks, the group of 114 students were divided into four quartile groups, based on their TA score. The 25% students with the highest TA score formed the quartile group Q4, the second 25% students the Q3 group, etc. The Q4 group included 16 of the 21 students involved in the teaching graphing formulas. All written type A tasks were analyzed on the main strategies, that is, the use of recognition/reasoning, making calculations, and “no answer at all” (blank). The written type B tasks were analyzed on the main strategies recognition/reasoning, making a graph, making calculations, and blank. For each task and each group (Q4, Q3, Q2, Q1), the relative frequencies of the main strategies and also the mean scores of the four groups on the tasks were calculated.

The thinking-aloud protocols could detail the main strategies that were used to analyze the written tasks. To analyze the thinking-aloud protocols, these were transcribed, and the transcripts were cut into idea units, fragments that contained crucial steps of explanations (Schwarz & Hershkowitz, 1999). These idea units were encoded using Drijvers et al.’s (2011) framework and descriptions of experts’ strategies in graphing formulas (Kop et al., 2015). Drijvers et al.’s framework uses the following categories: taking global view, reasoning, and strategic work. To detail the symbol sense in the category global view, strategies involved in graphing formulas were used: recognition of function families, using knowledge of prototypical graphs and other characteristics of the function family, and recognition of key features, without (instantly) knowing other characteristics. The category strategic work was split up: considering one’s strategy and monitoring and abandoning a representation (e.g., making a graph). Also, signs of lack of symbol sense were encoded; e.g., when time-consuming and error-prone algebraic calculations were used, while the problem could be solved with recognition and reasoning. This led to the following codebook for the type A and type B tasks that is explained in Table 1:

Table 1 Encoding the thinking-aloud protocols

We give three examples to illustrate the encoding.

As a first example, we consider student T working on task 14 (type B); they considered their strategy (S1), recognized the zeroes from the structure of the formula (R2), made a graph (S2), and used qualitative reasoning when y values are described in terms of positive/negative (Q):

Hmm, not nice to expand the brackets and to differentiate the function; but can it be done smarter? (S1); we can say that there will be a zero at 0, and when 14 − 2x = 0, so, at 7 and at 4 (R2); what shape do we have? (S2); for large x it is positive multiply negative multiply negative, so positive; for a very negative number we get a negative outcome (Q) (followed by a correct graph; score 1; encoding R2,Q,S1,S2).

As a second example, student A was working on task 4 (task B); they started with calculations (C), monitored their strategy (S1), recognized a key feature (asymptote) (R2), and used qualitative reasoning about function behavior in the neighborhood of x = 3 (Q).

First expand the brackets x2 − 13x + 30 + 40/(x − 3) (C); can this be larger than 70?; I’m going to try to find the turning point (S1); then see whether it is a parabola with a max or something like that, but there is also a broken function; let us see whether it is a parabola (R1) and see whether turning point is beneath or above 70 and then 40…..?(S1) (tries to calculate (C)) No, this will not work (S1); I think I calculate some points (S1); there is a vertical asymptote at x = 3 (R2); so, when x-3 is very small then this part becomes very large and dominate the rest of the function (Q); …x-3 can be infinity small and then the fraction will be very large and easily above 70 (Q) (score 1; encoding: R2,Q,S1,C)

In example 3, student Y was working on task 3 (task B); they used graphs (S2) and prototypical graphs (R1) and described a “reversed” prototypical graph (Q).

2x is equation of e(R1), so goes above (sketches a graph (S2); 2x goes the other direction (Q), so, they have 1 point of intersection (two correct graphs; score 1; encoding R1,Q,S2)

The categories to describe symbol sense in the codebook show some similarities with the Pierce and Stacey (2004) algebraic expectation framework. However, because our focus is on reading through formulas and making sense of them, the manipulation of formulas and equivalence of formulas plays a minor role compared with the Pierce and Stacey framework. The encoding was used to qualitatively study similarities and differences between the symbol sense use in the graphing and algebra tasks of each student.

4 Results

First, Table 2 shows the correlation between the variables Math rating, TA scores, TB, StratTB, and EffStratTB scores. The TA scores were strong correlated with the type B scores.

Table 2 Pearson’s correlation coefficients between variables, with 95% bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

Next, the Math rating was added as an independent variable in the regression model with TB score as dependent variable and TA score as independent variable and later also with the StratTB score and the EffStratTB score as dependent variables. Table 3 shows that this resulted in slightly higher correlation coefficients, .694, .543, and .639, respectively, than were found in Table 2.

Table 3 Linear model of predictors of type B scores, with 95% bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals

4.1 Students’ symbol sense in type A and type B tasks

Tables 4 and 5 show a more detailed picture of strategies of the Q4 and Q3 groups on the selection of type A and B tasks. The students in the groups Q1 and Q2 scored much lower on the use of symbol sense strategies than the Q3 and Q4 groups. Therefore, we only report on the Q3 and Q4 groups for a representative set of tasks. For type A tasks, we choose task 9 and 11 (working with parameters), task 15 (finding a formula), task 16 (graphing a formula), and task 19 (checking features of a graph). For type B tasks, we choose task 2 and 3 (number of solutions), task 4 (y > 70), task 5 (inequality), task 7 (y values), task 14 (about maximum), and task 18 (reasoning from formula). Tables 4 and 5 show that students of group Q4, as expected, used more symbol sense strategies than those in group Q3. See Tables 4 and 5.

Table 4 Strategy use (in percentages) of group Q3 and group Q4 on a selection of type A tasks
Table 5 Strategy use (in percentages) of group Q3 and group Q4 on a selection of type B tasks

In the other tasks that are not included in Table 5, also, the Q4 students used more symbol sense than the Q3 students, who at their turn did much better than the groups Q1 and Q2. The Q4 students also had higher mean scores than the Q3 group. Sometimes the differences in mean scores were very large, e.g., on task 1 (.57 vs .18), task 2 (.47 vs .13), task 3 (.49 vs .17), task 4 (.33 vs .16), task 7 (.84 vs .50), task 13 (.45 vs .22), and task 14 (.30 vs .00). However, we found exceptions, namely, the tasks about inequalities (task 5 and 6). In these tasks, the Q3 students used symbol sense strategies more often than the Q4 students, and in task 5, the inequality x(x − 1) > 4x, the Q3 students scored higher than the Q4 students (mean scores 0.70 versus 0.57). In the discussion, we discuss these findings about the inequalities.

To qualitatively back up the quantitative findings, the thinking-aloud protocols were analyzed according to the codebook of Table 1. Results of these analyses are presented in Table 6. In the columns “symbol sense on graphing tasks” and “symbol sense in algebra tasks,” the strategies that were predominantly used by a student are reported, that is, strategies used in more than 30% of the tasks. In Table 6, we used the following codes: R1, recognition of function families; R2, recognition of key graph features; Q, qualitative reasoning; S1, considering a strategy and monitoring; S2, abandoning a representation (e.g., making a graph); and C, calculation.

Table 6 Scores and strategy use of the six thinking-aloud students

The results in Table 6 seem to confirm the findings of the quantitative analyses of Tables 2 and 3, showing a relation between the scores on the graphing and algebra tasks. Table 6 shows that students often used recognition and qualitative reasoning when working on both kinds of tasks. As expected, the S2 strategy (abandoning a representation) was used more often in the algebra tasks than in the graphing tasks. Apart from the S2 strategy, there was some relation between the strategies used in both types of tasks; only students A and M showed a larger difference in strategy use between both kinds of tasks, as they often started with calculations working on the algebra tasks. In Appendix 2, for each student, illustrative transcripts with encodings plus samples of their written work are given. It shows that, in both tasks, the students often needed combinations of recognition, reasoning, and strategic work to solve the tasks. However, using more strategies was not always an indication of proficiency. For instance, the high achieving student K was short in their reasoning, using function families and qualitative reasoning.

5 Discussion and conclusion

In this study, we investigated how students’ graphing by hand abilities might be related to symbol sense abilities to solve non-routine algebra tasks. We designed a symbol sense test with graphing problems (type A tasks) and other algebra tasks that could be solved with graphs and reasoning, without algebraic calculation (type B tasks).

With respect to the first sub-question about the relation between the graphing formulas abilities and the abilities to solve the algebra problems with symbol sense, we found that the students who scored better on the graphing tasks also scored higher on the algebra tasks. General mathematics abilities might explain this relation. However, when Math rating was added as an independent variable, the explained variance of the scores on the algebra tasks hardly increased. This suggested a positive relationship between students’ graphing abilities and their abilities to solve algebra tasks. A similar positive relationship was found between the scores on the graphing tasks and the symbol sense scores on the algebra tasks (StratTB and EffStratTB scores), indicating that students who scored higher on the graphing tasks used more and more effectively symbol sense strategies while solving the algebra tasks. This relation was confirmed by the analyses between the Q3 and Q4 groups in Tables 4 and 5, which showed that the Q4 students used more symbol sense strategies than the Q3 students.

The second sub-question was about similarities and differences between symbol sense use in the graphing and algebra tasks. In the analyses of the thinking-aloud protocols, we found that the six students often used similar symbol sense strategies in both the graphing and algebra tasks. Students’ approaches to solve the graphing tasks could be described through combinations of recognition of function families and using prototypical graphs and characteristics, recognition of key features of the function, and qualitative reasoning. To these combinations, the strategy “abandoning a representation” (making a graph) was added, when working on the algebra tasks. The high-scoring students more often used “making a graph” and had a larger repertoire of symbol sense strategies than the other students, who more often tried to use calculations, and had trouble to use combinations of strategies. The findings suggest that, besides “make a graph,” students often used similar strategies in the graphing and algebra tasks.

The study aimed to contribute to the knowledge of symbol sense and to the students’ symbol sense abilities in graphing formulas and in non-routine algebra tasks. With respect to the main research question on how students’ graphing by hand abilities might be related to their symbol sense use while solving non-routine algebra tasks, our findings suggest that students could use their symbol sense involved in graphing formulas, that is, a combination of recognition of function families and graph features from the structure of the function, qualitative reasoning, and strategic work, to solve algebra tasks.

5.1 Limitations

Before discussing these results in more detail, we acknowledge that the study, of course, also came with limitations. The algebra tasks in our test were restricted to problems, predominantly using the variables x and y, that could be solved with graphs and reasoning, without algebraic calculation. Another issue is the combination of graphing and algebra tasks in one test which might have given suggestions to use graphs in the type B tasks. In a future study, these issues could be addressed by omitting explicit graphing tasks, by also using other variables than x and y, and by adding some tasks that need some algebraic calculation. In this article, the focus was on the relation between graphing abilities and the symbol sense abilities to solve algebra tasks. A next step would be to set up a quasi-experimental study, in which a group of students were taught to graph formulas by hand, using a control group and a pre-test and post-test.

In discussing the findings, we note that the results of this study seem to confirm earlier research about the problems Dutch students have with algebra: students have problems graphing formulas by hand (Kop et al., 2020) and with identifying and using the structure of algebraic expressions (Van Stiphout, Drijvers, & Gravemeijer, 2013). Regular teaching of algebra does not seem to develop these aspects of symbol sense. Although only Dutch students were involved in this study, literature about symbol sense (Arcavi et al., 2017; Drijvers et al., 2011; Kieran, 2006; Arcavi, 1994; Ayalon, Watson, & Lerman, 2015; Hoch & Dreyfus, 2005, 2010; Oehrtman et al., 2008) and personal conversations with teachers and scholars from other countries suggest that grade 12 students abroad have similar problems with symbol sense.

A remarkable finding, described in the “Results” section, was that in task 5, the inequality, the Q3 students scored higher and did use more symbol sense strategies than the Q4 students, who more often tried calculations to solve this task. We wonder why the Q4 students did not use their graphing skills in this task, as we expect that they could easily graph both formulas. Although we know from literature that students may over rely on the symbolic representation even when graphs are more appropriate (Knuth, 2000; Eisenberg & Dreyfus, 1994; Kenney, 2008; Slavit, 1997), we assume that the inequality triggered previously learned associations that hinder later learned symbol sense, as was suggested by student Y (see Appendix 2).

5.2 Implications

The findings of the current study suggest a positive relationship between the ability to graph formulas by hand and to solve non-routine algebra tasks and showed similarities in the symbol sense used in both kinds of tasks. The contribution of this study is that it describes this symbol sense through combinations of recognition of function families and key graph features from the structure of the formulas, qualitative reasoning, and strategic work and that it suggests how this symbol sense might be taught to students.

Graphing formulas and covariational reasoning, in the context of formulas, are related as both have a focus on global (qualitative) graphs. In this study, it is explicitly shown how and what qualitative reasoning was used by students. The importance of qualitative reasoning and its omission in regular mathematics education have been stressed by Goldenberg, Lewis, and O’Keefe (1992), Yerushalmy (1997), and Duval (2006). In their elaborations about covariational reasoning, Moore and Thompson (2015) have problematized what they called static shape thinking that is seeing a graph-as-a-wire. However, our findings show that the students successfully used prototypical graphs of function families as building blocks in their reasoning, when working on the graphing tasks and using the strategy “making a graph” in the algebra tasks. The need for such repertoire of functions that can be instantly visualized by a graph has been stressed by many, for example, by Eisenberg and Dreyfus (1994), Stylianou and Silver (2004), and Duval (2006). In combination with qualitative reasoning, such repertoire might provide a knowledge base that is needed to enable students using visualizations to solve algebra problems (“making a graph” strategy). Visualization is more than just making or perceiving graphs, it is about noticing and understanding the whole that is represented with its features, for which a solid knowledge base is needed (Duval, 2006).

Teaching symbol sense is not easy (Arcavi et al., 2017; Hoch & Dreyfus, 2005). Before we describe our suggestions about teaching symbol sense, we discuss extant approaches. Pierce and Stacey (2007) suggested highlighting the formula’s structure and key features in classroom discussions when working with graphs. Friedlander and Arcavi (2012) focused on meaningful reading of algebraic formulas and formulated small tasks that focused on, e.g., qualitative thinking and global comprehension. Kindt (2011) gave many examples of productive practice in algebra. These activities are valuable and can be easily added to existing lessons, but often manipulations of formulas play a central role in these activities, and they lack a systematic and a step-by-step development. Our approach to teaching symbol sense focuses on enabling students to make sense of formulas and to read through formulas. In literature, it has been suggested that giving meaning to formulas can be done via linking representations of functions and/or via realistic contexts (Kieran, 2006). However, except for linear and exponential functions, formulas often cannot be directly linked to realistic contexts. Therefore, we choose to link formulas to graphs through graphing formulas. To learn about functions, many have recommended to use technology to link representations (Kieran, 2006; Kieran & Drijvers, 2006; Heid, Thomas, & Zbiek, 2013). However, Goldenberg (1988) found that students established the connection between formula and graph more effectively when they drew graphs by hand than when they only performed computer graphing. Others have recognized the need for pen-and-paper activities when working with technology (Arcavi et al., 2017; Kieran & Drijvers, 2006). Therefore, we tried to promote students’ symbol sense through graphing formulas by hand.

In this study, we found strong correlations between students’ graphing by hand abilities and their abilities to solve algebra tasks and their use of symbol sense while solving non-routine algebra tasks. These correlations could not be accounted for by students’ general mathematics abilities. Symbol sense involved in graphing formulas includes combinations of recognition of function families and key features from the structure of formulas and (qualitative) reasoning, and it is a subset of symbol sense involved in solving non-routine algebra tasks. In the current study, 16 of the 21 students who were involved in the teaching of graphing formulas by hand belonged to the 25% highest scoring students on the graphing tasks (Q4 group), who used more symbol sense when solving algebra tasks than the other students. The six thinking-aloud students, all involved in the teaching and belonging to the Q4 group, showed that they were able to use their symbol sense and graphing formulas abilities to solve the non-routine algebra tasks, that is, combinations of recognition, qualitative reasoning, and strategic work. This suggests that teaching symbol sense in the domain of graphing formulas by hand might be an effective means to teach essential aspects of symbol sense involved in solving non-routine algebra tasks. In a previous study, we showed how to teach graphing formulas by hand, using these essential aspects of symbol sense (Kop et al., 2020).

The current study provides more insight in the relation between symbol sense involved in graphing formulas by hand and in solving non-routine algebra tasks, in what aspects of symbol sense students can use while solving non-routine algebra tasks, and how this symbol sense might be taught. However, more research is needed to investigate how educational practices might benefit from these insights. Also, lower grades of secondary school could be included in such research, investigating Ruthven’s suggestion to start algebra with graphing activities, instead of algebraic calculations (Ruthven, 1990), and our suggestion to learn about functions through a combination of graphing tools to explore functions and graphing by hand activities to foster students’ symbol sense.