Skip to main content
Log in

True-False Testing on Trial: Guilty as Charged or Falsely Accused?

  • Intervention Study
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although widely used, the true-false test is often regarded as a superficial or even harmful test, one that lacks the pedagogical efficacy of more substantive tests (e.g., cued-recall or short-answer tests). Such charges, however, lack conclusive evidence and may, in some cases, be false. Across four experiments, we investigated how true-false testing of studied passages (e.g., on Yellowstone National Park) might enhance—or be optimized to enhance—performance on subsequent cued-recall tests. In Experiments 1–2, relative to control performance that did not benefit from any additional exposure, we found that (a) the evaluation of true statements enhanced the recall of tested (but not related) content and that (b) the evaluation of false statements enhanced the recall of related (but not tested) content, a differential pattern of benefits that did not depend on the syntactic structure of the test items. Moreover, when competitive clauses were embedded within the true-false items of Experiment 3 (e.g., True or false? Castle Geyser (not Steamboat Geyser) is the tallest geyser), we found that the evaluation of both types of statements enhanced the recall of both types of content. Finally, in Experiment 4, these holistic benefits proved robust to a retention interval of 48 h and were comparable with the benefits of a restudy condition in which learners restudied all of the propositions that could have been retrieved in the evaluation of the true-false items. Accordingly, although it was not uncommon for participants to misremember information as a consequence of true-false practice, our findings broadly indicate that, especially when carefully constructed, true-false tests can elicit beneficial, not superficial, processes that belie their poor reputation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Longman, D. J. A. (1978). The influence of length and frequency of training session on the rate of learning to type. Ergonomics, 21(8), 627–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modifier: an interpretation of negative recency and related phenomena. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blasiman, R. N., Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2017). The what, how much, and when of study strategies: comparing intended versus actual study behaviour. Memory, 25(6), 784–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. C., & Roediger III, H. L. (2008). Feedback enhances the positive effects and reduces the negative effects of multiple-choice testing. Memory & Cognition, 36(3), 604–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (1992). The influence of retrieval on retention. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 633–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cocks, A. W. (1929). The pedagogical value of the true-false examination. Baltimore: Warwick & York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, S. M. (1992). True-false, alternate-choice, and multiple-choice items. Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice, 11(3), 27–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, D., & Bjork, R. A. (1994). Learning, remembering, believing: enhancing human performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebel, R. L. (1970). The case for true-false test items. The School Review, 78(3), 373–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foss, D. J., & Pirozzolo, J. W. (2017). Four semesters investigating frequency of testing, the testing effect, and transfer of training. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(8), 1067–1083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glover, J. A. (1989). The "testing" phenomenon: not gone but nearly forgotten. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 392–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzberg, O. E., Heilman, J. D., & Leuenberger, H. W. (1932). The value of objective tests as teaching devices in educational psychology classes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 23(5), 371–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jersild, A. T. (1929). Examination as an aid to learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 20(8), 602–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, A. C., Wardlow, L., Pan, S. C., Zepeda, C., Heyman, G. D., Dunlosky, J., & Rickard, T. C. (2016). Beyond the rainbow: retrieval practice leads to better spelling than does rainbow writing. Educational Psychology Review, 28(2), 385–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keys, N. (1934). The influence of true-false items on specific learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 25(7), 511–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, J. L., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Angello, G. (2012). Multiple-choice tests exonerated, at least of some charges: fostering test-induced learning and avoiding test-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1337–1344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, S. C., & Rickard, T. C. (2017). Does retrieval practice enhance learning and transfer relative to restudy for term-definition facts? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23(3), 278–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pan, S. C., & Rickard, T. C. (2018). Transfer of test-enhanced learning: meta-analytic review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 144(7), 710–756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remmers, H. H., & Remmers, E. M. (1926). The negative suggestion effect on true-false examination questions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 17(1), 52–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, H. M., & Ruch, G. M. (1928). Minor studies on objective examination methods. The Journal of Educational Research, 18(2), 112–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger III, H. L., & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 20–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger III, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). Test-enhanced learning: taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger III, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). The power of testing memory: basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(3), 181–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger III, H. L., Agarwal, P. K., Kang, S. H. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2010). Benefits of testing memory: best practices and boundary conditions. In G. M. Davies & D. B. Wright (Eds.), New frontiers in applied memory (pp. 13–49). Brighton, U.K.: Psychology Press.

  • Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: a meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1432–1463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sproule, C. E. (1934). Suggestion effects of the true-false test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 25(4), 281–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storey, A. G. (1966). A review of evidence or the case against the true-false item. The Journal of Educational Research, 59(6), 282–285.

  • Toppino, T. C., & Brochin, H. A. (1989). Learning from tests: the case of true-false examinations. The Journal of Educational Research, 83(2), 119–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toppino, T. C., & Cohen, M. S. (2009). The testing effect and the retention interval: questions and answers. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 252–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toppino, T. C., & Luipersbeck, S. M. (1993). Generality of the negative suggestion effect in objective tests. The Journal of Educational Research, 86(6), 357–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venn, J. (1884). Studies and exercises in formal logic, including a generalisation of logical processes in their application to complex inferences. Mind, 9(34), 301–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yue, C. L., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). Reducing verbal redundancy in multimedia learning: an undesired desirable difficulty ? Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 266–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors were involved in the conception and design of the study. J.A.B. generated initial hypotheses, programmed all experiments, coordinated their execution, and analyzed and interpreted the data. J.A.B. and S.C.P. drafted the manuscript with input from E.L.B. and R.A.B. All authors approved the manuscript for submission.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jordan Andrew Brabec.

Ethics declarations

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Office of the Human Research Protection Program at the University of California, Los Angeles (IRB# 11-002880) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(DOCX 16 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brabec, J.A., Pan, S.C., Bjork, E.L. et al. True-False Testing on Trial: Guilty as Charged or Falsely Accused?. Educ Psychol Rev 33, 667–692 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09546-w

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09546-w

Keywords

Navigation