Skip to main content
Log in

A Case for Domain-Specific Curiosity in Mathematics

  • REVIEW ARTICLE
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Epistemic curiosity is a desire for knowledge accompanied by positive emotions, increased arousal, and exploratory behavior (Grossnickle, Educational Psychology Review, 28(1), 23–60, 2016). Although curiosity has typically been characterized as a domain-general construct, domain-general conceptualizations do not acknowledge systematic changes in an individuals’ development (e.g., domain knowledge) as they advance within a domain. Moreover, a domain-general conceptualization of curiosity stands in direct contrast to research on interest, given that interest is typically described as domain-specific (e.g., interest for mathematics). Without a domain-specific conceptualization of curiosity as it relates to development within academic domains, comparisons between curiosity and interest will remain muddled. In the present theoretical review, we put forward a conceptualization of curiosity as domain-specific and examine how the components of curiosity develop within one academic domain: mathematics. In doing so, we juxtapose conceptualizations of epistemic curiosity with literature related to the development of other epistemic factors (i.e., knowledge, epistemic beliefs) in mathematics. Specifically, we build on the knowledge gap theories of epistemic curiosity (Litman, Personality and Individual Differences, 48(4), 397–410, 2010; Loewenstein, Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 75–98, 1994) to consider developmental shifts in (a) knowledge gaps, (b) heightened arousal, and (c) exploratory behaviors within the domain of mathematics. Understanding the domain-specific and developmental nature of curiosity is critical for distinguishing curiosity from interest and for supporting motivation within mathematics classrooms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is important to note that curiosity is not universally viewed as positive. Some researchers have argued that curiosity has an aversive component, whereby individuals seek to reduce the discomfort of uncertainty (Berlyne 1960; Jepma et al. 2012; Litman and Jimerson 2004). However, more research is needed (see Murayama et al. this issue).

References

  • Ainley, M. D. (1987). The factor structure of curiosity measures: breadth and depth of interest curiosity styles. Australian Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 53–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainley, M., & Ainley, J. (2011). Student engagement with science in early adolescence: the contribution of enjoyment to students’ continuing interest in learning about science. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 4–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainley, M., Hidi, S., & Berndorff, D. (2002). Interest, learning, and the psychological processes that mediate their relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(3), 545–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, P. A. (1997). Mapping the multidimensional nature of domain learning: the interplay of cognitive, motivational, and strategic forces. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 213–250). Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, P. A., & Grossnickle, E. M. (2016). Positioning interest and curiosity within a model of academic development. In K. Wentzel & D. Miele (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (2nd ed., pp. 188–208). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, R. S., D’Mello, S. K., Rodrigo, M. M. T., & Graesser, A. C. (2010). Better to be frustrated than bored: the incidence, persistence, and impact of learners’ cognitive–affective states during interactions with three different computer-based learning environments. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 68(4), 223–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baranes, A., Oudeyer, P. Y., & Gottlieb, J. (2015). Eye movements reveal epistemic curiosity in human observers. Vision Research, 117, 81–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 20–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail: working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science, 16(2), 101–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boaler, J. (2008). What’s math got to do with it?: helping children learn to love their most hated subject—and why it’s important for America. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boaler, J. (2015). Mathematical mindsets: unleashing students’ potential through creative math, inspiring messages and innovative teaching. San Francisco: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boaler, J., & Selling, S. K. (2017). Psychological imprisonment or intellectual freedom? A longitudinal study of contrasting school mathematics approaches and their impact on adults’ lives. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 48(1), 78–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bôcher, M. (1904). The fundamental conceptions and methods of mathematics. Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society, 11(3), 115–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehme, K. L., Goetz, T., & Preckel, F. (2017). Is it good to value math? Investigating mothers’ impact on their children’s test anxiety based on control-value theory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 11–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. L., & Newton, K. J. (2012). Fractions: could they really be the gatekeeper’s doorman? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 37(4), 247–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, L. (2010). The self-regulation of curiosity and interest during the information search process of adolescent students. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 61(7), 1332–1344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyle, G. J. (1989). Breadth-depth or state-trait curiosity? A factor analysis of state-trait curiosity and state anxiety scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 10(2), 175–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 602–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. I., & Walter, M. I. (2004). The art of problem posing. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byman, R. (1993). From two-dimensional to three-dimensional curiosity: a reanalysis of depth-breadth factor model. Australian Journal of Psychology, 45(3), 155–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chak, A. (2007). Teachers’ and parents’ conceptions of children’s curiosity and exploration. International Journal of Early Years Education, 15(2), 141–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T., & VanLehn, K. A. (2012). Seeing deep structure from the interactions of surface features. Educational Psychologist, 47(3), 177–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • College Board (2018). Compare SAT specifications. Retrieved 10 September from https://collegereadiness.collegeboard.org/sat/inside-the-test/compare-old-new-specifications

  • Common Core State Standards (2018). Standards for mathematic practice. Retrieved 10 September 2018 from http://www.corestandards.org/Math/Practice/

  • Cromley, J. G., Booth, J. L., Wills, T. W., Chang, B. L., Tran, N., Madeja, M., Shipley, T. F., & Zahner, W. (2017). Relation of spatial skills to calculus proficiency: a brief report. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 19(1), 55–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Mello, S., & Graesser, A. (2012). Dynamics of affective states during complex learning. Learning and Instruction, 22(2), 145–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeVilliers, M. (1999). Rethinking proof with the Geometer’s sketchpad. Berkeley: Key Curriculum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1910). How we think. New York: D. C. Heath & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dumas, D., Alexander, P. A., & Grossnickle, E. M. (2013). Relational reasoning and its manifestations in the educational context: a systematic review of the literature. Educational Psychology Review, 25(3), 391–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweighter, H. (1975). Elementary problem E2569. The American Mathematical Monthly, 82, 1010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel, S. (2015). The hungry mind: the origins of curiosity in childhood. Boston: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, A. E., Herts, J. B., Borgonovi, F., Guerriero, S., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2017). The math anxiety-performance link: a global phenomenon. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(1), 52–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Dicke, A. L., & Goetz, T. (2012). Beyond quantitative decline: conceptual shifts in adolescents’ development of interest in mathematics. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1069–1082.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. (2010). Bootstrapping the mind: analogical processes and symbol systems. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 752–775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M., Camp, C. J., & Grodsky, A. (1992). Curiosity and stimulation seeking across the adult life span: cross-sectional and 6- to 8-year longitudinal findings. Psychology and Aging, 7(1), 150–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15(1), 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Givvin, K. B., Stigler, J. W., & Thompson, B. J. (2011). What community college developmental mathematics students understand about mathematics, part II: the interviews. The MathAMATYC Educator, 2(3), 4–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: an intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24(6), 645–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. (2006). A history of prizes in mathematics. In J. Carlson, A. Jaffe, & A. Wiles (Eds.), The millennium prize problems (pp. 3–30). Providence: The American Mathematical Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossnickle, E. M. (2014). The expression and enactment of interest and curiosity in a multiple source use task (Doctoral dissertation).

  • Grossnickle, E. M. (2016). Disentangling curiosity: dimensionality, definitions, and distinctions from interest in educational contexts. Educational Psychology Review, 28(1), 23–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, M. J., Gelman, B. D., & Ranganath, C. (2014). States of curiosity modulate hippocampus-dependent learning via the dopaminergic circuit. Neuron, 84(2), 486–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Beilock, S. L., & Levine, S. C. (2012). The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: the role of the linear number line. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1229–1241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, N., Jordan, N. C., Fernandez, E., Siegler, R. S., Fuchs, L., Gersten, R., & Micklos, D. (2015). General and math-specific predictors of sixth-graders’ knowledge of fractions. Cognitive Development, 35, 34–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental resource for learning. Review of Educational Research, 60(4), 549–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 42, 111–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, B. K., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67(1), 88–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Imm, K., & Stylianou, D. A. (2012). Talking mathematically: an analysis of discourse communities. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 31(1), 130–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iran-Nejad, A. (1990). Active and dynamic self-regulation of learning processes. Review of Educational Research, 60(4), 573–602.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iran-Nejad, A., & Cecil, C. (1992). Interest and learning: a biofunctional perspective. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 297–332). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73(2), 509–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jepma, M., Verdonschot, R. G., Van Steenbergen, H., Rombouts, S. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the induction and relief of perceptual curiosity. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(5), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jirout, J., & Klahr, D. (2012). Children’s scientific curiosity: in search of an operational definition of an elusive concept. Developmental Review, 32(2), 125–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jirout, J., Vitiello, V. E., & Zumbrunn, S. K. (2018). Curiosity in schools. In G. Gordon (Ed.), The new science of curiosity (pp. 243–265). New York: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 850–867.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., Wang, J. T.-Y., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). The wick in the candle of learning: epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory. Psychological Science, 20(8), 963–973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., Rose, P., & Fincham, F. D. (2004). Curiosity and exploration: facilitating positive subjective experiences and personal growth opportunities. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82(3), 291–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., Gallagher, M. W., Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Breen, W. E., Terhar, D., & Steger, M. F. (2009). The curiosity and exploration inventory-II: development, factor structure, and psychometrics. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 987–998.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., Stiksma, M. C., Disabato, D. J., McKnight, P. E., Bekier, J., Kaji, J., & Lazarus, R. (2018). The five-dimensional curiosity scale: capturing the bandwidth of curiosity and identifying four unique subgroups of curious people. Journal of Research in Personality, 73, 130–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellman, P. J., Massey, C. M., & Son, J. Y. (2010). Perceptual learning modules in mathematics: enhancing students’ pattern recognition, structure extraction, and fluency. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(2), 285–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knobloch, S., Patzig, G., Mende, A. M., & Hastall, M. (2004). Affective news: effects of discourse structure in narratives on suspense, curiosity, and enjoyment while reading news and novels. Communication Research, 31(3), 259–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knuth, E. J. (2002). Fostering mathematical curiosity. The Mathematics Teacher, 95(2), 126–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (2002). Structural and dynamic aspects of interest development: theoretical considerations from an ontogenetic perspective. Learning and Instruction, 12(4), 383–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamnina, M., & Chase, C. C. (2019). Developing a Thirst for Knowledge: How Uncertainty in the Classroom Influences Curiosity, Affect, Learning, and Transfer. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 59, 1–13.

  • Larkin, K., & Jorgensen, R. (2016). ‘I hate maths: why do we need to do maths?’ Using iPad video diaries to investigate attitudes and emotions towards mathematics in year 3 and year 6 students. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 14(5), 925–944.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ligneul, R., Mermillod, M., & Morisseau, T. (2018). From relief to surprise: dual control of epistemic curiosity in the human brain. NeuroImage, 181, 490–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Durik, A. M., Conley, A. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Karabenick, S. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Measuring situational interest in academic domains. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70(4), 647–671.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litman, J. (2005). Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: wanting and liking new information. Cognition & Emotion, 19(6), 793–814.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litman, J. A. (2008). Interest and deprivation factors of epistemic curiosity. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(7), 1585–1595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litman, J. A. (2010). Relationships between measures of I- and D-type curiosity, ambiguity tolerance, and need for closure: an initial test of the wanting-liking model of information seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(4), 397–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litman, J. A., & Jimerson, T. L. (2004). The measurement of curiosity as a feeling of deprivation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82(2), 147–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litman, J. A., & Spielberger, C. D. (2003). Measuring epistemic curiosity and its diversive and specific components. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80(1), 75–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litman, J. A., Hutchins, T. L., & Russon, R. K. (2005). Epistemic curiosity, feeling-of-knowing, and exploratory behaviour. Cognition and Emotion, 19(4), 559–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 75–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luce, M. R., & Hsi, S. (2015). Science-relevant curiosity expression and interest in science: an exploratory study. Science Education, 99(1), 70–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology, 8(5), 656–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Mathematics anxiety: separating the math from the anxiety. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2102–2110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, E. A., Schaeffer, M. W., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Mathematics anxiety and stereotype threat: shared mechanisms, negative consequences and promising interventions. Research in Mathematics Education, 15(2), 115–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hřebíčková, M., Avia, M. D., Sanz, J., Sánchez-Bernardos, M. L., Kusdil, M. E., Woodfield, R., Saunders, P. R., & Smith, P. B. (2000). Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 173–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrea, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Conceptions and correlates to openness to experience. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 826–848). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGillivray, S., Murayama, K., & Castel, A. D. (2015). Thirst for knowledge: the effects of curiosity and interest on memory in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 30(4), 835–841.

    Google Scholar 

  • Misfeldt, M., & Johansen, M. W. (2015). Research mathematicians’ practices in selecting mathematical problems. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 89(3), 357–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morsanyi, K., McCormack, T., & O'Mahony, E. (2018). The link between deductive reasoning and mathematics. Thinking & Reasoning, 24(2), 234–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 33–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muenks, K., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2018). I can do this! The development and calibration of children’s expectations for success and competence beliefs. Developmental Review, 48, 24–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muis, K. R. (2004). Personal epistemology and mathematics: a critical review and synthesis of research. Review of Educational Research, 74(3), 317–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muis, K. R., Bendixen, L. D., & Haerle, F. C. (2006). Domain-generality and domain-specificity in personal epistemology research: philosophical and empirical reflections in the development of a theoretical framework. Educational Psychology Review, 18(1), 3–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muis, K. R., Pekrun, R., Sinatra, G. M., Azevedo, R., Trevors, G., Meier, E., & Heddy, B. C. (2015a). The curious case of climate change: testing a theoretical model of epistemic beliefs, epistemic emotions, and complex learning. Learning and Instruction, 39, 168–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muis, K. R., Psaradellis, C., Lajoie, S. P., Di Leo, I., & Chevrier, M. (2015b). The role of epistemic emotions in mathematics problem solving. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 42, 172–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muis, K. R., Trevors, G., Duffy, M., Ranellucci, J., & Foy, M. J. (2016). Testing the TIDE: examining the nature of students’ epistemic beliefs using a multiple methods approach. The Journal of Experimental Education, 84(2), 264–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muis, K. R., Chevrier, M., & Singh, C. A. (2018). The role of epistemic emotions in personal epistemology and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 3, 165–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murayama, K., FitzGibbon, L., & Sakaki, M. (this issue). Process account of curiosity and interest: A reward-learning perspective. Educational Psychology Review.

  • Murphy, P. K., Firetto, C. M., & Li, M. (2018). Knowledge and the model of domain learning. In H. Fives & D. L. Dinsmore (Eds.), The model of domain learning: understanding the development of expertise (pp. 19–36). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2018). Standards and positions: process. Retrieved 10 September 2018 from https://www.nctm.org/Standards-and-Positions/Principles-and-Standards/Process/

  • National Research Council (2001). Adding it up: helping children learn mathematics. In J. Kilpatrick, J. Swafford, & B. Findell (Eds.), Mathematics Learning Study Committee, Center for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

  • Nook, E. C., Sasse, S. F., Lambert, H. K., McLaughlin, K. A., & Somerville, L. H. (2018). The nonlinear development of emotion differentiation: granular emotional experience is low in adolescence. Psychological Science, 19(80), 1346–1357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noordewier, M. K., & van Dijk, E. (2017). Curiosity and time: from not knowing to almost knowing. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 411–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, D., Tsukayama, E., Goodwin, G. P., Patrick, S., & Duckworth, A. L. (2017). A tripartite taxonomy of character: evidence for intrapersonal, interpersonal, and intellectual competencies in children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 48, 16–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 18(4), 315–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Vogl, E., Muis, K. R., & Sinatra, G. M. (2017). Measuring emotions during epistemic activities: the epistemically-related emotion scales. Cognition and Emotion, 31(6), 1268–1276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, R. A. (1978). Effects of anxiety, curiosity, and perceived instructor threat on student verbal behavior in the college classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(3), 388–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, E. G., Alexander, P. A., & List, A., (2016). The argument for epistemic competence. In B. Moschner, A. Anschütz, & H. Gruber (Eds.), Wissen und Lernen: Wie epistemische Überzeugungen Schule, Universität und Arbeitswelt beeinflussen (Knowledge and learning in the perspective of learners and instructor: how epistemic beliefs influence school, university, and the workplace). Berlin: Waxmann Verlag.

  • Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: a review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(2), 110–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramani, G. B., & Eason, S. H. (2015). It all adds up: learning early math through play and games. Phi Delta Kappan, 96(8), 27–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reio, T. G., Jr., & Wiswell, A. (2000). Field investigation of the relationship among adult curiosity, workplace learning, and job performance. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 11(1), 5–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reio, T. G., Jr., Petrosko, J. M., Wiswell, A. K., & Thongsukmag, J. (2006). The measurement and conceptualization of curiosity. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167(2), 117–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, K. A. (2000). Individual interest and its implications for understanding intrinsic motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: the search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 373–404). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2016). The power of interest for motivation and learning. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B. (1988). Treating mathematics as an ill-structured discipline. In R. I. Charles & E. A. Silver (Eds.), The teaching and assessing of mathematical problem solving: research agenda for mathematics education series (Vol. 3). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richland, L. E., Zur, O., & Holyoak, K. J. (2007). Cognitive supports for analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science, 316(5828), 1128–1129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richland, L. E., Stigler, J. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (2012). Teaching the conceptual structure of mathematics. Educational Psychologist, 47(3), 189–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B. (2017). Developing mathematics knowledge. Child Development Perspectives, 11(3), 184–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., & Alibali, M. W. (1999). Conceptual and procedural knowledge of mathematics: does one lead to the other? Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 175–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, O. C., Demetre, J. D., & Litman, J. A. (2017). Adult life stage and crisis as predictors of curiosity and authenticity: testing inferences from Erikson’s lifespan theory. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41(3), 426–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 54–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2006). Maintaining activity engagement: individual differences in the process of self-regulating motivation. Journal of Personality, 74(6), 1697–1720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory mechanism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 379–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U. (2009). Situational and individual interest. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 197–222). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1988). When good teaching leads to bad results: the disasters of well-taught mathematics courses. Educational Psychologist, 23(2), 145–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(3), 498–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schommer, M., & Walker, K. (1995). Are epistemological beliefs similar across domains? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), 424–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Lehman, S. (2001). Situational interest: a review of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), 23–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sher, K. B. T., Levi-Keren, M., & Gordon, G. (2019). Priming, enabling and assessment of curiosity. Educational Technology Research and Development, 67(4), 931–952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., & Zhou, X. (2013). Fractions: the new frontier for theories of numerical development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 13–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sierpinska, A., Bobos, G., & Knipping, C. (2008). Sources of students’ frustration in pre-university level, prerequisite mathematics courses. Instructional Science, 36(4), 289–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silver, E. A. (1994). On mathematical problem posing. For the Learning of Mathematics, 14(1), 19–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion, 5(1), 89–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2008). Appraisal components and emotion traits: examining the appraisal basis of trait curiosity. Cognition and Emotion, 22(1), 94–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J., Henson, R. A., & Templin, J. L. (2009). Are the sources of interest the same for everyone? Using multilevel mixture models to explore individual differences in appraisal structures. Cognition and Emotion, 23(7), 1389–1406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skagerlund, K., & Träff, U. (2016). Processing of space, time, and number contributes to mathematical abilities above and beyond domain-general cognitive abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 85–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevors, G. J., Muis, K. R., Pekrun, R., Sinatra, G. M., & Muijselaar, M. M. (2017). Exploring the relations between epistemic beliefs, emotions, and learning from texts. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 48, 116–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ufer, S., Rach, S., & Kosiol, T. (2017). Interest in mathematics = interest in mathematics? What general measures of interest reflect when the object of interest changes. ZDM, 49(3), 397–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schijndel, T. J., Jansen, B. R., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2018). Do individual differences in children’s curiosity relate to their inquiry-based learning? International Journal of Science Education, 40(9), 996–1015.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P., & Hommel, B. (2011). Threat but not arousal narrows attention: evidence from pupil dilation and saccade control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(281), 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verdine, B. N., Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Newcombe, N. S., Filipowicz, A. T., & Chang, A. (2014). Deconstructing building blocks: preschoolers’ spatial assembly performance relates to early mathematical skills. Child Development, 85(3), 1062–1076.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watt, H. M. (2004). Development of adolescents’ self-perceptions, values, and task perceptions according to gender and domain in 7th- through 11th-grade Australian students. Child Development, 75(5), 1556–1574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., Siegler, R. S., & Davis-Kean, P. E. (2014). What’s past is prologue: relations between early mathematics knowledge and high school achievement. Educational Researcher, 43(7), 352–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wavo, E.-Y.-T. (2004). Honesty, cooperation, and curiosity achievement of some schools on Nanjing (China). IFE PsychologIA, 12, 178–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, K., Inglis, M., & Mejia-Ramos, J. P. (2014). How mathematicians obtain conviction: implications for mathematics instruction and research on epistemic cognition. Educational Psychologist, 49(1), 36–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weible, J. L., & Zimmerman, H. T. (2016). Science curiosity in learning environments: developing an attitudinal scale for research in schools, homes, museums, and the community. International Journal of Science Education, 38(8), 1235–1255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, P. H., Kuo, C. Y., Wu, H. K., Jen, T. H., & Hsu, Y. S. (2018). Learning benefits of secondary school students’ inquiry-related curiosity: a cross-grade comparison of the relationships among learning experiences, curiosity, engagement, and inquiry abilities. Science Education, 102(5), 917–950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: when students believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47(4), 302–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, C. J., Levine, S. C., & Mix, K. S. (2018). The connection between spatial and mathematical ability across development. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(755), 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emily Grossnickle Peterson.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Peterson, E.G., Cohen, J. A Case for Domain-Specific Curiosity in Mathematics. Educ Psychol Rev 31, 807–832 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09501-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09501-4

Keywords

Navigation