Advertisement

Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 24, Issue 3, pp 449–469 | Cite as

Using Fictional Sources in the Classroom: Applications from Cognitive Psychology

  • Elizabeth J. MarshEmail author
  • Andrew C. Butler
  • Sharda Umanath
Review Article

Abstract

Fictional materials are commonly used in the classroom to teach course content. Both laboratory experiments and classroom demonstrations illustrate the benefits of using fiction to help students learn accurate information about the world. However, fictional sources often contain factually inaccurate content, making them a potent vehicle for learning misinformation about the world. We briefly review theoretical issues relevant to whether learners process fictional sources differently before exploring how individual differences, learning activities, and assessment characteristics may affect learning from fiction. This review focuses on our own experimental approaches for studying learning from fiction, including learning from short stories and from films, while connecting to a broader educational literature on learning from fictional sources. Throughout the review, implications for educational use and future directions for experimental research are noted.

Keywords

Fiction Learning Education Knowledge Suggestibility 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a collaborative award from the James S. McDonnell Foundation. We thank Meredith Mechanik for assistance with manuscript preparation.

References

  1. Appel, M. (2008). Fictional narratives cultivate just-world beliefs. Journal of Communication, 58, 62–83.Google Scholar
  2. Appel, M., & Richter, T. (2007). Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. Media Psychology, 10, 113–134.Google Scholar
  3. Anderson, J. R. (1981). Effects of prior knowledge on memory for new information. Memory & Cognition, 9, 237–246.Google Scholar
  4. Ausubel, D. P., & Blake, E. (1958). Proactive inhibition in the forgetting of meaningful school material. The Journal of Educational Research, 52, 145–149.Google Scholar
  5. Barber, S. J., Rajaram, S., & Marsh, E. J. (2008). Distinguishing among remembered, familiar, and just known memories: An experimental analysis. Memory, 16, 934–946.Google Scholar
  6. Barnett, M., & Kafka, A. (2007). Using science fiction movie scenes to support critical analysis of science. Journal of College Science Teaching, 36, 31–35.Google Scholar
  7. Bauer, P. J., & San Souci, P. (2010). Going beyond the facts: Young children extend knowledge by integrating episodes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 452–465.Google Scholar
  8. Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classification of educational goals. New York: David McKay Company Inc.Google Scholar
  9. Bottoms, H. C., Eslick, A. N., & Marsh, E. J. (2010). Memory and the Moses illusion: Failures to detect contradictions with stored knowledge yielded negative memorial consequences. Memory, 18, 670–678.Google Scholar
  10. Bowen, J. (1972). A history of Western education. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  11. Brabham, E., Boyd, P., & Edgington, W. (2000). Sorting it out: Elementary students’ responses to fact and fiction in informational storybooks as read-alouds for science and social studies. Reading Research and Instruction, 29(4), 265–290.Google Scholar
  12. Brophy, J., & VanSledright, B. (1997). Teaching and learning history in elementary schools. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
  13. Butler, A. C., Dennis, N. A., & Marsh, E. J. (2012). Inferring facts from fiction: Reading correct and incorrect information affects memory for related information. Memory, 20, 487–498.Google Scholar
  14. Butler, A. C., Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). The effect of type and timing of feedback on learning from multiple-choice tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 273–281.Google Scholar
  15. Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Feedback enhances the positive effects and reduces the negative effects of multiple-choice testing. Memory & Cognition, 36, 604–616.Google Scholar
  16. Butler, A. C., Zaromb, F., Lyle, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2009). Using popular films to enhance classroom learning: The good, the bad, and the interesting. Psychological Science, 20, 1161–1168.Google Scholar
  17. Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 306–307.Google Scholar
  18. Chiesi, H. L., Spilich, G. J., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Acquisition of domain-related information in relation to high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 257–274.Google Scholar
  19. Collett, J. L., Kelly, S., & Sobolewski, C. (2010). Using Remember the Titans to teach theories of conflict reduction. Teaching Society, 38, 258–266.Google Scholar
  20. Coté, N., Goldman, S. R., & Saul, E. U. (1998). Students making sense of informational text: Relations between processing and representation. Discourse Processes, 25, 1–53.Google Scholar
  21. Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.Google Scholar
  22. Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.Google Scholar
  23. Daisey, P. (1994). The value of trade books in secondary science and mathematics instruction: A rationale. School Science and Mathematics, 94, 130–137.Google Scholar
  24. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.Google Scholar
  25. Dempster, F. N. (1989). Spacing effects and their implications for theory and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 309–330.Google Scholar
  26. Dooling, D. J., & Mullet, R. L. (1973). Locus of thematic effects in retention of prose. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97, 404–406.Google Scholar
  27. Dubeck, L. W., Moshier, S. E., & Boss, J. E. (2004). Fantastic voyages: Learning science through science fiction films. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  28. Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Owen, P. D., & Coté, N. C. (1990). Encoding and recall of texts: The importance of material appropriate processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 566–581.Google Scholar
  29. Elliott, D. L., & Woodward, A. (Eds.). (1990). Textbooks and schooling in the United States: Eighty-ninth yearbook of the National Society of Education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  30. Erickson, T. D., & Mattson, M. E. (1981). From words to meaning: A semantic illusion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 549–551.Google Scholar
  31. Eslick, A. N., Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2011). Ironic effects of drawing attention to story errors. Memory, 19, 184–191.Google Scholar
  32. Fazio, L. K., Barber, S. J., Rajaram, S., Ornstein, P. A., & Marsh, E. J. (2012). Creating illusions of knowledge: Learning errors that contradict prior knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi: 10.1037/a0028649.
  33. Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2008a). Older, not younger, children learn more from false facts from stories. Cognition, 106, 1081–1089.Google Scholar
  34. Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2008b). Slowing presentation time increases, rather than decreases, errors learned from fictional stories. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 180–185.Google Scholar
  35. Friesen, N. (2011). The lecture as a transmedial pedagogical form: A historical analysis. Educational Research, 40, 95–102.Google Scholar
  36. Ganea, P. A., Pickard, M. B., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Transfer between picture books and the real world by very young children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9, 46–66.Google Scholar
  37. Garner, R., Gillingham, M. G., & White, C. S. (1989). Effects of “seductive details” on macroprocessing and microprocessing in adults and children. Cognition and Instruction, 6, 41–57.Google Scholar
  38. Gerrig, R. J. (1989). Suspense in the absence of uncertainty. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 633–648.Google Scholar
  39. Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds: On the psychological activities of reading. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Gerrig, R. J., & Prentice, D. A. (1991). The representation of fictional information. Psychological Science, 2, 336–340.Google Scholar
  41. Gerrig, R. J., & Rapp, D. N. (2004). Psychological processes underlying literary impact. Poetics Today, 25, 265–281.Google Scholar
  42. Goswick, A. E., Mullet, H. G., & Marsh, E. J. (2012). Age differences in suggestibility: Can production difficulties and source monitoring explain a developmental reversal? Journal of Cognition and Development. (in press).Google Scholar
  43. Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 701–721.Google Scholar
  44. Green, M. C. (2006). Narratives and cancer communication. Journal of Communication, 56, S163–S183.Google Scholar
  45. Green, M. C., Brock, T. C., & Kaufman, G. F. (2004). Understanding media enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative worlds. Communication Theory, 14, 311–327.Google Scholar
  46. Green, M. C., Garst, J., & Brock, T. C. (2004). The power of fiction: Determinants and boundaries. In The psychology of entertainment media: blurring the lines between entertainment and persuasion. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  47. Green, M. C., Garst, J., Brock, T. C., & Chung, S. (2006). Fact versus fiction labeling: Persuasion parity despite heightened scrutiny of fact. Media Psychology, 8, 267–285.Google Scholar
  48. Greene, E., Flynn, M., & Loftus, E. (1982). Inducing resistance to misleading information. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 21, 207–219.Google Scholar
  49. Grove, M. (2000). Warning: This culture will self-destruct in 5 seconds (p. 16). London: The Times.Google Scholar
  50. Harp, S. F., & Mayer, R. E. (1998). How seductive details do their damage: A theory of cognitive interest in science learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 414–434.Google Scholar
  51. Haynes, C., & Richgels, D. (1992). Fourth graders’ literature preferences. The Journal of Educational Research, 85, 208–219.Google Scholar
  52. Hoffman, H., Granhag, P., See, S., & Loftus, E. (2001). Social influences on reality-monitoring decisions. Memory & Cognition, 29, 394–404.Google Scholar
  53. Howe, M. L. (1991). Misleading children’s story recall: Forgetting and reminiscence of the facts. Developmental Psychology, 27, 746–762.Google Scholar
  54. Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15, 635–650.Google Scholar
  55. Janit, A. S., Hammock, G. S., & Richardson, D. S. (2011). The power of fiction: Reading stories in abnormal psychology. International Journal for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 5, 1–14.Google Scholar
  56. Jenkins, J. J. (1979). Four points to remember: A tetrahedral model of memory experiments. In L. S. Cermak & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing and human memory. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  57. Kennedy, F. N., Senses, N., & Ayan, P. (2011). Grasping the social through movies. Teaching in Higher Education, 16, 1–14.Google Scholar
  58. Kole, J. A., & Healy, A. F. (2007). Using prior knowledge to minimize interference when learning large amounts of information. Memory & Cognition, 35, 124–137.Google Scholar
  59. Lewis, C. H., & Anderson, J. R. (1976). Interference with real world knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 311–335.Google Scholar
  60. Lorch, E. P., Sanchez, R. P., van den Broek, P., Milich, R., Murphy, E. L., Lorch, R. J., Jr., & Welsh, R. (1999). The relation of story structure properties to recall of television stories in young children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and nonreferred peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 293–309.Google Scholar
  61. Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 173–192.Google Scholar
  62. Marcus, A. S. (2005). “It is as it was”: Feature film in the history classroom. The Social Studies, 96, 61–67.Google Scholar
  63. Marcus, A. S. (2007). Celluloid blackboard: Teaching history with film. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  64. Marcus, A., & Monaghan, M. (2009). Tasting the fluoride: Feature film, women’s history, and modern-day critical consciousness. Social Studies Research & Practice, 4, 1–30.Google Scholar
  65. Marcus, A. S., Paxton, R. J., & Meyerson, P. (2006). “The reality of it all”: History students read the movies. Theory and Research in Social Education, 34, 516–552.Google Scholar
  66. Marsh, E. J. (2004). Story stimuli for creating false beliefs about the world. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 650–655.Google Scholar
  67. Marsh, E. J., & Fazio, L. K. (2006). Learning errors from fiction: Difficulties in reducing reliance on fictional stories. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1140–1149.Google Scholar
  68. Marsh, E. J., & Fazio, L. K. (2007). Learning from fictional sources. In J. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger III (pp. 397–413). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  69. Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 519–536.Google Scholar
  70. Mayer, D. A. (1995). How can we best use literature in teaching? Science and Children, 32, 16–19.Google Scholar
  71. McDaniel, M. A., Blischak, D., & Einstein, G. O. (1995). Understanding the special mnemonic characteristics of fairy tales. In C. A. Weaver, III, S. Mannes, & C. R. Fletcher (Eds.), Discourse comprehension: Essays in honor of Walter Kintsch (pp. 157–176). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  72. McDaniel, M. A., & Butler, A. C. (2010). A contextual framework for understanding when difficulties are desirable. In A. S. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: Essays in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 175–199). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  73. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1989). Material-appropriate processing: A contextualist approach to reading and studying strategies. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 113–145.Google Scholar
  74. McDaniel, M. A., Hines, R. J., Waddell, P. J., & Einstein, G. O. (1994). What makes folk tales unique: Content familiarity, causal structure, scripts, or superstructures? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 169–184.Google Scholar
  75. Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1980). Norms of 300 general-information questions: Accuracy of recall, latency of recall, and feeling-of-knowledge ratings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 338–368.Google Scholar
  76. Niemi, R. (2006). History in the media: Film and television. Santa Barbara: ABC-CLIO.Google Scholar
  77. Nokes, J. D. (2008). Aligning literary practices in secondary history classes with research on learning. Literary Practices in History, 3, 29–55.Google Scholar
  78. O’Connor, J. E. (1987). Teaching history with film and television. Washington: American Historical Association.Google Scholar
  79. Owens, C. V. (2003). Nonsense, sense, and science: Misconceptions and illustrated trade books. Journal of Children’s Literature, 29, 55–62.Google Scholar
  80. Palmer, J., & Burroughs, S. (2002). Integrating children’s literature and song into the social studies. The Social Studies, 93, 73–78.Google Scholar
  81. Peshkam, A., Mensink, M. C., Putnam, A. L., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Warning readers to avoid irrelevant information: When being vague might be valuable. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 219–231.Google Scholar
  82. Peterson, S. B., & Potts, G. R. (1982). Global and specific components of information integration. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 403–420.Google Scholar
  83. Potts, G. R., St. John, M. F., & Kirson, D. (1989). Incorporating new information into existing world knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 303–333.Google Scholar
  84. Prentice, D. A., Gerrig, R. J., & Bailis, D. S. (1997). What readers bring to the processing of fictional texts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 416–420.Google Scholar
  85. Rapp, D. N. (2008). How do readers handle incorrect information during reading? Memory & Cognition, 36, 688–701.Google Scholar
  86. Rice, D. C. (2002). Using trade books in teaching elementary science: Facts and fallacies. The Reading Teacher, 55, 552–565.Google Scholar
  87. Robinson, C., Larsen, J., Haupt, J., & Mohlman, J. (1997). Picture book selection behaviors of emergent readers: Influence of genre, familiarity, and book attributes. Reading Research and Instruction, 36, 287–304.Google Scholar
  88. Roediger, H. L., III. (2000). Why retrieval is the key process to understanding human memory. In E. Tulving (Ed.), Memory, consciousness and the brain: The Tallinn conference (pp. 52–75). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  89. Roediger, H. L., III, & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 20–27.Google Scholar
  90. Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181–210.Google Scholar
  91. Rose, C. (2003). How to teach biology using the movie science of cloning people, resurrecting the dead and combining flies and humans. Public Understanding of Science, 12, 289–296.Google Scholar
  92. Roser, N., & Keehn, S. (2002). Fostering thought, talk, and inquiry: Linking literature and social studies. The Reading Teacher, 55, 416–426.Google Scholar
  93. Royce, C. A., & Wiley, D. A. (1996). Children’s literature and the teaching of science: Possibilities and cautions. Clearing House, 70, 18–20.Google Scholar
  94. Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and counting-out rhymes. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  95. Sadoski, M. (2001). Resolving the effects of concreteness on interest, comprehension, and learning important ideas from text. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 263–281.Google Scholar
  96. Saldana, J. (2008). Popular film as an instructional strategy in qualitative research methods courses. Qualitative Inquiry, 15, 247–261.Google Scholar
  97. Sanchez, C. A., & Wiley, J. (2006). An examination of the seductive details effect in terms of working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 34, 344–355.Google Scholar
  98. Schacter, D. L., Harbluk, J. L., & McLachlan, D. R. (1984). Retrieval without recollection: An experimental analysis of source amnesia. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 593–611.Google Scholar
  99. Schaffer, L. S. (1927). A learning experiment in the social studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 18, 557–591.Google Scholar
  100. Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (2005). Zoning-out during reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children (pp. 204–226). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  101. Shafto, M., & MacKay, D. G. (2000). The Moses, mega-Moses, and Armstrong illusions: Integrating language comprehension and semantic memory. Psychological Science, 11, 372–378.Google Scholar
  102. Shafto, M. A., & MacKay, D. G. (2010). Miscomprehension, meaning, and phonology: The unknown and phonological Armstrong illusions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 529–568.Google Scholar
  103. Short, G. (1997). Learning through literature: Historical fiction, autobiography, and the holocaust. Children’s Literature in Education, 28, 179–190.Google Scholar
  104. Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion, 5, 89–102.Google Scholar
  105. Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  106. Silvia, P. J., & Berg, C. (2011). Finding movies interesting: How appraisals and expertise influence the aesthetic experience of film. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 29, 73–88.Google Scholar
  107. Smith, J. A. (1993). Content learning: A third reason for using literature in teaching reading. Reading, Research, and Instruction, 32, 64–71.Google Scholar
  108. Smith, G. W. (2009). Using feature films as the primary instructional medium to teach organizational behavior. Journal of Management Education, 33, 462–489.Google Scholar
  109. Smith, V. L., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1987). The social psychology of eyewitness accuracy: Misleading questions and communicator expertise. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 294–300.Google Scholar
  110. Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1993). Where does knowledge come from? Specific associations between print exposure and information acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 211–229.Google Scholar
  111. Stevens, B. (2003). American history through feature films: A middle school unit. Film & History, 33, 65–69.Google Scholar
  112. Trundle, K. C., Troland, T. H., & Pritchard, T. G. (2008). Representations of the Moon in children’s literature: An analysis of written and visual text. Journal of Elementary Science Education, 20, 17–28.Google Scholar
  113. Umanath, S., Butler, A. C., & Marsh, E. J. (2012). Positive and negative effects of monitoring popular films for historical inaccuracies. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 556–567.Google Scholar
  114. Umanath, S. & Marsh, E. J. (2012). Aging and the memorial consequences of catching contradictions with prior knowledge. Psychology & Aging. doi: 10.1037/a0027242.
  115. Underwood, J., & Pezdek, K. (1998). Memory suggestibility as an example of the sleeper effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 136–141.Google Scholar
  116. Van den Broek, P., Virtue, S., Everson, M. G., Tzeng, Y., & Sung, Y. (2002). Comprehension and memory of science texts: Inferential processes and the construction of a mental representation. In The psychology of science text comprehension. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  117. Ventura, S., & Onsman, A. (2009). The use of popular movies during lectures to aid the teaching and learning of undergraduate pharmacology. Medical Teacher, 31, 662–664.Google Scholar
  118. Wheeler, S. C., Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (1999). Fictional narratives change beliefs: Replications of Prentice, Gerrig, and Bailis (1997) with mixed corroboration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 136–141.Google Scholar
  119. Wolfe, M. B. W. (2005). Memory for narrative and expository text: Independent influences of semantic associations and text organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 359–364.Google Scholar
  120. Wolfe, M. B. W., & Mienko, J. A. (2007). Learning and memory of factual content from narrative and expository text. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 541–564.Google Scholar
  121. Zwaan, R. A. (1994). Effects of genre expectations on text comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 920–933.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elizabeth J. Marsh
    • 1
    Email author
  • Andrew C. Butler
    • 1
  • Sharda Umanath
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychology & NeuroscienceDuke UniversityDurhamUSA

Personalised recommendations