Skip to main content

Auditory Sensitivity, Speech Perception, and Reading Development and Impairment

Abstract

While the importance of phonological sensitivity for understanding reading acquisition and impairment across orthographies is well documented, what underlies deficits in phonological sensitivity is not well understood. Some researchers have argued that speech perception underlies variability in phonological representations. Others have investigated the role of more general auditory sensitivity for reading development and reading difficulties, arguing that poor phonological representations may actually be due to broad underlying auditory deficits, which are not restricted to speech stimuli. We argue that these hypotheses are not necessarily mutually exclusive. In this review, we demonstrate that auditory sensitivity and speech perception can be integrated into a single developmental model, in which auditory sensitivity may have an indirect impact on reading; this impact is mediated by speech perception. In the model, we distinguish general auditory sensitivity as falling into at least two general categories: rhythmic and temporal. Correspondingly, speech perception itself can be distinguished as suprasegmental and segmental. Theoretically, the proposed model integrates a broad range of studies on general auditory and speech perception to suggest a developmental trajectory for reading acquisition that can be explored from before birth. Practically, the proposed model points to different ways of understanding and diagnosing reading difficulties and distinguishing reading difficulties across languages and orthographies.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahissar, M. (2007). Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 458–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahissar, M., Protopapas, A., Reid, M., & Merzenich, M. M. (2000). Auditory processing parallels reading abilities in adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 6832–6837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ahissar, M., Lubin, Y., Putter-Katz, H., & Banai, K. (2006). Dyslexia and the failure to form a perceptual anchor. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1558–1564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amitay, S., Ahissar, M., & Nelken, I. (2002). Auditory processing deficits in reading disabled adults. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 3, 302–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, M. E., Munson, B., & Edwards, J. (2007). Vocabulary growth and the developmental expansion of types of phonological knowledge. In J. Cole & J. Hualde (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 9 (pp. 242–264). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biermann, S., & Heil, P. (2000). Parallels between timing of onset responses of single neurons in cat and of evoked magnetic Welds in human auditory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 2426–2439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogliotti, C., Serniclaes, W., Messaoud-Galusi, S., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2008). Discrimination of speech sounds by children with dyslexia: Comparisons with chronological age and reading level controls. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 137–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., Chung, K. K. H., Wong, S. W. L., McBride-Chang, C., Penney, T. B., & Ho, C.-S. H. (2008). Perception of tone and aspiration contrasts in Chinese children with dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 726–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clin, E., Wade-Woolley, L., & Heggie, L. (2009). Prosodic sensitivity and morphological awareness in children's reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 197–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corriveau, K., Pasquini, E., & Goswami, U. (2007). Basic auditory processing skills and specific language impairment: A new look at an old hypothesis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 647–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (1990). Exploiting prosodic probabilities in speech segmentation. In G. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 105–121). Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Carter, D. M. (1987). The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech & Language, 2, 133–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeBree, E., Wijnen, F., & Zonneveld, W. (2006). Word stress production in three-year-old children at risk of dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 304–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fang, Z. (1990). Tone sandhi and tone perception. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 22, 255–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 460–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foxton, J. M., Talcott, J. B., & Witton, J. B. (2003). Reading skills are related to global, but not local, acoustic pattern perception. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 343–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fry, D. B. (1954). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26, 138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: Towards a cross linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia, 6, 133–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2002). Phonology, reading development, and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 141–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Thomson, J. M., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 10911–10916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Gerson, D., & Astru, L. (2009). Amplitude envelope perception, phonology and prosodic sensitivity in children with developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. http://www.springerlink.com/content/eqk2318618670m36/. Accessed May 10, 2010.

  • Hämäläinen, J., Leppänen, P. H. T., Torppa, M., Müller, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Detection of sound rise time by adults with dyslexia. Brain and Language, 94, 32–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hämäläinen, J. A., Leppänen, P. H., Guttorm, T. K., & Lyytinen, H. (2008). Event-related potentials to pitch and rise time change in children with reading disabilities and typically reading children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, 100–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heil, P. (2003). Coding of temporal onset envelope in the auditory system. Speech Communication, 41, 123–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2008). Sensitivity to speech rhythm explains individual differences in reading ability independently of phonological awareness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 357–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W. (1992). Developing phonological categories from the speech signal. In C. A. Ferguson, L. Menn, & C. Stoel-Gammon (Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications. Timonium, MD: York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kujala, T., Belitz, S., Tervaniemi, M., & Näätänen, R. (2003). Auditory sensory memory disorder in dyslexic adults as indexed by the mismatch negativity. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1323–1327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, W., Shu, H., & Yang, Y. (2009). Speech perception deficits by Chinese children with phonological dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 338–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Guttorm, T. K., Huttunen, T., Hämäläinen, J., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Vesterinen, M. (2005). Psychophysiology of developmental dyslexia: A review of findings including studies of children at risk for dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 167–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., & Keating, P. (2005). Speech perception in dyslexic children with and without language impairments. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 77–99). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M. S., Keating, P., Doi, L. M., Munson, B., et al. (1997). Are speech perception deficits associated with developmental dyslexia? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 211–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, U., Bucher, K., Brem, S., & Brandeis, D. (2003). Altered responses to tone and phoneme mismatch in kindergartners at familial dyslexia risk. NeuroReport, 14, 2245–2250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1995). Phonological processing, speech perception and reading disability: An integrative review. Educational Psychologist, 30, 109–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1996). Models of speech perception and phonological processing in reading. Child Development, 67, 1836–1856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., et al. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Lam, F., Lam, C., Doo, S., Wong, S. W. L., & Chow, Y. Y. Y. (2008). Word recognition and cognitive profiles of Chinese pre-school children at risk for dyslexia through language delay or familial history of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 211–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Tong, X., Shu, H., Wong, A. M.-Y., Leung, K., & Tardif, T. (2008). Syllable, phonemes, and tone: Psycholinguistic units in early Chinese and English word recognition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 171–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Sai, X., Wang, C., Wang, J., Sha, S., & Zhou, X. (2005). Auditory and speech processing and reading development in Chinese school children: Behavioural and ERP evidence. Dyslexia, 11, 292–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Brady, S. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: Auditory processing or phonological coding? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 199–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moisescu-Yiflach, T., & Pratt, H. (2005). Auditory event related potentials and source current density estimation in phonologic/auditory dyslexics. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116, 2632–2647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. L. (2000). Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain and Language, 72, 238–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. L., Molfese, V. J., & Modglin, A. (2004). Development of auditory event-related potentials in young children and relations to word-level reading abilities at age 8 years. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 9–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muneaux, M., Ziegler, J. C., Truc, C., Thomson, J., & Goswami, U. (2004). Deficits in beat perception and dyslexia: Evidence from French. NeuroReport, 15, 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, U., Thomson, J., Scott, S. K., & Goswami, U. (2004). Supra-segmental auditory processing skills and phonological representation in dyslexic children. Dyslexia, 10, 215–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rvachew, S., & Grawburg, M. (2006). Correlates of phonological awareness in preschoolers with speech sound disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 74–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., & Remschmidt, H. (1998). Auditory processing and dyslexia: Evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. NeuroReport, 9, 337–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Carre, R., & Demonet, J.-F. (2001). Perceptual discrimination of speech sounds in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 384–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11, 171–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talcott, J. B., Gram, A., van Ingelhem, M., Witton, C., Stein, J. F., & Toennessen, F. E. (2003). Impaired sensitivity to dynamic stimuli in poor readers of a regular orthography. Brain and Language, 87, 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, J. M., Fryer, B., Maltby, J., & Goswami, U. (2006). Auditory and motor rhythm awareness in adults with dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 334–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, J. M., Goswami, U., & Baldeweg, T. (2009). The ERP signature of sound rise time changes. Brain Research, 1254, 74–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition: Cross language and writing system transfer. Cognition, 97, 67–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whalley, K., & Hansen, J. (2006). The role of prosodic sensitivity in children's reading development. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 288–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C. (2006). Metrical stress sensitivity in young children and its relationship to phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 270–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C., & Terrell, C. (1998). Poor readers' ability to detect speech rhythm and perceive rapid speech. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 397–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C., Wade-Woolley, L., & Holliman, A. J. (2009). Prosodic awareness: Beyond phonemes. In C. Wood & V. Connelly (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on reading and spelling (pp. 9–23). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Y. (1991). Depth of phonological recording in short-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 19, 263–273.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

We are grateful to Dr. Benjamin Munson for his suggestions on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Catherine McBride-Chang.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zhang, J., McBride-Chang, C. Auditory Sensitivity, Speech Perception, and Reading Development and Impairment. Educ Psychol Rev 22, 323–338 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9137-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9137-4

Keywords

  • Auditory sensitivity
  • Speech perception
  • Reading