Abstract
In this article, themost relevant literature on cognitive aging and instructional design is merged to formulate recommendations for designing computer-based training material aimed at elderly learners. The core message is that researchers and instructional designers do not need to develop special computerized instruction for older adults. Rather, existing principles of general instructional and multimedia design can be evaluated and used to accommodate the needs of elderly learners. Particular attention is given to John Sweller’s Cognitive Load Theory (CLT) and Richard Mayer’s Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning (CTML). It is argued that these instructional theories bear important benefits for older learners because they support an efficient use of available cognitive resources. New research directions are suggested to test the implications of these theories for learning in old age.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


Notes
Solution to the water-jug problem in Fig. 1: The five units of water in the right working jug are poured into the left working jug, which has a capacity of seven units and can thus receive two more units of water. If the right jug is refilled and poured into the left jug, three units remain in the right jug. These three units are poured into the left jug after the left jug is emptied. Thus, the left jug can contain four more units of water. If the right jug is refilled with five units and is then poured into the left jug, one unit remains in the right jug. This is exactly the required amount of water which is subsequently poured into the target jug.
References
Atwood, M.E., Masson, M.E., & Polson, P.G. (1980). Further explorations with a process model for water jug problems. Memory and Cognition, 8, 182–192.
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.
Baltes, P.B., & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 12, 12–21.
Braver, T.S., & Barch, D.M. (2002). A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 809–817.
Burns, B.D., & Vollmeyer, R. (2002). Goal specificity effects on hypothesis testing in problem solving. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 241–261.
Caplan, L.J., & Schooler, C. (1990). The effects of analogical training models and age on problem-solving in a new domain. Experimental Aging Research, 16, 151–154.
Camp, G., Paas, F., Rikers, R., & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2001). Dynamic problem selection in air traffic control training: A comparison between performance, mental effort and mental efficiency. Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 575–595.
Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information-processing rate. In Birren, J.E., & Schaie, K.W. (eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 201–221). San Diego: Academic.
Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1992). The spit-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 233–246.
Charness, N. (2001). Aging and communication: Human factors issues. In Charness, N., Park, D.C., & Sabel, B.A. (eds.), Communication, technology, and aging: Opportunities and challenges for the future (pp. 1–29). New York: Springer.
Charness, N., Kelley, C. L., Bosman, E.A., & Mottram, M. (2001). Word-processing training and retraining: Effects of adult age, experience, and interface. Psychology and Aging, 16, 110–127.
Clark, J.M., & Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education. Educational Psychology Review, 3, 149–210.
Craik, F.I.M. (1986). A functional account of age differences in memory. In Klix, F., & Hagendorf, H. (eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities: Mechanisms and performances (Part A) (pp. 409–422). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Czaja, S.J. (1996). Aging and the acquisition of computer skills. In Rogers, W.A., Fisk, A.D., & Walker, N. (eds.), Aging and skilled performance: Advances in theory and applications (pp. 201–220). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Czaja, S.J., & Sharit, J. (1993). Age differences in the performance of computer-based work. Psychology and Aging, 8, 59–67.
Czaja, S.J., & Sharit, J. (1998a). Age differences in attitudes toward computers. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 53B, P329–P340.
Czaja, S.J., & Sharit, J. (1998b). Ability–performance relationships as a function of age and task experience for a data entry task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 332–351.
Czaja, S.J., Sharit, J., Ownby, R., Roth, D.L., & Nair, S. (2001). Examining age differences in performance of a complex information search and retrieval task. Psychology and Aging, 16, 564–579.
Daselaar, S.M., & Cabeza, R. (2005). Age-related changes in hemispheric organization. In Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D. (eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging (pp. 325–353). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Ericsson, K.A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.
Fisk, A.D., & Rogers, W.A. (2002). Psychology and aging: Enhancing the lives of an aging population. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 107–110.
Fisk, A.D., Rogers, W.A., Charness, N., Czaja, S.J., & Sharit, J. (2004). Designing for older adults: Principles and creative human factors approaches. Boca Raton, FL: CRC.
Fisk, J.E., & Sharp, C.A. (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning: Updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 874–890.
Fisk, J.E., & Warr, P. (1996). Age and working memory: The role of perceptual speed, the central executive, and the phonological loop. Psychology and Aging, 11, 316–323.
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R.T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In Bower, G.H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego: Academic Press.
Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 87–97.
Jamieson, B.A., & Rogers, W.A. (2000). Age-related effects of blocked and random practice schedules on learning a new technology. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 55B, P343–P353.
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1999). Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 351–371.
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2004). When redundant on-screen text in multimedia technical instruction can interfere with learning. Human Factors, 46, 567–581.
Leahy, W., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). When auditory presentations should and should not be a component of multimedia instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 401–418.
Luchins, A.S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54 (whole no. 248).
Mautone, P.D., & Mayer, R.E. (2001). Signaling as a cognitive guide in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 377–389.
Mayer, R.E. (2001). Multimedia learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Mayer, R.E. (ed.) (2005). The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mayer, R.E., & Anderson, R.B. (1992). The instructive animation: Helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 444–452.
Mayer, R.E., & Chandler, P. (2001). When learning is just a click away: Does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 390– 397.
Mayer, R.E., Dow, G.T., & Mayer, S. (2003). Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: What works in the design of agent-based microworlds? Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 806–813.
Mayer, R.E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 187–198.
Mayer, R.E., Mathias, A., & Wetzell, K. (2002). Fostering understanding of multimedia messages through pre-training: Evidence for a two-stage theory of mental model construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 147–154.
Mayer, R.E., Mautone, P., & Prothero, W. (2002). Pictorial aids for learning by doing in a multimedia geology simulation game. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 171–185.
Mayer, R.E., & Moreno, R. (1998). A split-attention effect in multimedia learning: Evidence for dual processing systems in working memory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 312–320.
Mayer, R.E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 43–52.
Mayer, R.E., Moreno, R., Boire, M., & Vagge, S. (1999). Maximizing constructivist learning from multimedia communications by minimizing cognitive load. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 638–643.
Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (1993). Sequential and coordinative complexity: Age-based processing limitations in figural transformations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1297–1320.
Mayr, U., Kliegl, R., & Krampe, R.T. (1996). Sequential and coordinative processing dynamics in figural transformations across the life span. Cognition, 59, 61–90.
Moreno, R., & Mayer, R.E. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 358–368.
Moreno, R., & Mayer, R.E. (2000). A coherence effect in multimedia learning: The case for minimizing irrelevant sounds in the design of multimedia instructional messages. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 117–125.
Morrell, R.W., & Echt, K.V. (1996). Instructional design for older computer users: The influence of cognitive factors. In Rogers, W.A., Fisk, A.D., & Walker, N. (eds.), Aging and skilled performance: Advances in theory and applications (pp. 241–265). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Morrell, R.W., & Echt, K.V. (1997). Designing written instructions for older adults: Learning to use computers. In Fisk, A.D., & Rogers, W.A. (eds.), Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 335–361). San Diego: Academic Press.
Morrell, R.W., & Park, D.C. (1993). The effects of age, illustrations, and task variables on the performance of procedural assembly tasks. Psychology and Aging, 8, 389–399.
Morris, J.M. (1994). Computer training needs of older adults. Educational Gerontology, 20, 541–555.
Mousavi, S.Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 319–334.
Oberauer, K., & Kliegl, R. (2001). Beyond resources: Formal models of complexity effects and age differences in working memory. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 187–215.
Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429–434.
Paas, F., Camp, G., & Rikers, R. (2001). Instructional compensation for age-related cognitive declines: Effects of goal specificity in maze learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 181–186.
Paas, F., Tuovinen, J.E., Tabbers, H.K., & Van Gerven, P.W.M. (2003). Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist, 38, 63–71.
Paas, F., Van Gerven, P.W.M., & Tabbers, H.K. (2005). The cognitive aging principle in multimedia learning. In Mayer, R.E. (ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 339–351). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Paas, F., & Van Merrienboer, J.J.G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.
Park, D.C. (1994). Aging, cognition, and work. Human Performance, 7, 181–205.
Perfect, T.J., & Maylor, E.A. (2000). Rejecting the dull hypothesis: The relation between method and theory in cognitive aging research. In Perfect, T.J., & Maylor, E.A. (eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 1–18). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Persad, C.C., Abeles, N., Zacks, R.T., & Denburg, N.L. (2002). Inhibitory changes after age 60 and their relationship to measures of attention and memory. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, P223–P232.
Pirolli, P.L., & Anderson, J.R. (1985). The role of learning from examples in the acquisition of recursive programming skills. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 240–272.
Rogers, W.A., & Fisk, A.D. (2000). Human factors, applied cognition, and aging. In Craik, F.I.M., & Salthouse, T.A. (eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 559–591). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rowe, J.W., & Kahn, R.L. (1987). Human aging: Usual and successful. Science, 237, 143–149.
Salden, R.J.C.M., Paas, F., Broers, N.J., & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Mental effort and performance as determinants for the dynamic selection of learning tasks in air traffic control training. Instructional Science, 32, 153–172.
Salthouse, T.A. (1992). Why do adult age differences increase with task complexity? Developmental Psychology, 28, 905–918.
Salthouse, T.A. (1993). Speed mediation of adult age differences in cognition. Developmental Psychology, 29, 722–738.
Salthouse, T.A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.
Satz, P. (1993). Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology, 7, 273–295.
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448–460.
Sweller, J. (1999). Instructional design in technical areas. Camberwell, Australia: ACER Press.
Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12, 185–233.
Sweller, J., & Levine, M. (1982). Effects of goal specificity on means-ends analysis and learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 463–474.
Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.
Tabbers, H.K., Martens, R.L., & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Multimedia instructions and cognitive load theory: Effects of modality and cueing. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 71–81.
Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). When two sensory modes are better than one. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 257–287.
Tisserand, D.J., & Jolles, J. (2003). On the involvement of prefrontal networks in cognitive ageing. Cortex, 39, 1107–1128.
Van Gerven, P.W.M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J. J.G., Hendriks, M., & Schmidt, H.G. (2003). The efficiency of multimedia learning into old age. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 489–505.
Van Gerven, P.W.M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Schmidt, H.G. (2000). Cognitive load theory and the acquisition of complex cognitive skills in the elderly: Towards an integrative framework. Educational Gerontology, 26, 503–521.
Van Gerven, P.W.M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Schmidt, H.G. (2002). Cognitive load theory and aging: Effects of worked examples on training efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 38, 87–107.
Van Gerven, P.W.M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Schmidt, H.G. (2006). Modality and variability as factors in training the elderly. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 311–320.
Van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177.
Ward, M., & Sweller, J. (1990). Structuring effective worked examples. Cognition and Instruction, 7, 1–39.
Zhu, X., & Simon, H.A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137–166.
Acknowledgments
We thank three anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments on the first version of this article.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Van Gerven, P.W.M., Paas, F. & Tabbers, H.K. Cognitive Aging and Computer-Based Instructional Design: Where Do We Go From Here?. Educ Psychol Rev 18, 141–157 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9005-4
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9005-4
Keywords
- Cognitive aging
- Instructional design
- Complex skill learning
- Cognitive load
- Multimedia