Advertisement

Early Childhood Education Journal

, Volume 46, Issue 6, pp 629–642 | Cite as

The Efficacy of Multimedia Stories in Preschoolers’ Explicit and Implicit Story Comprehension

  • Dilek AltunEmail author
Article

Abstract

Oral language provides a foundation for reading comprehension. Story comprehension is a fundamental oral language skill; it covers making inferences, identifying main ideas, monitoring, perspective-taking, and applying working memory capacity. Complex reasoning and perspective-taking are key factors in deep reading comprehension. Preliterate children’s deeper story comprehension skills can be initial indicators of their later reading comprehension. Thus, the purpose of this research is to investigate preliterate preschool children’s story comprehension skills in detail. This study focuses on the additional multimedia features of digital storybooks and whether they hinder or promote young children’s explicit and implicit comprehension in a small group reading activity. The findings revealed that (a) children in the multimedia-enhanced storybook group outperformed the print storybook group in terms of both explicit and implicit story comprehension, (b) explicit story comprehension was higher than implicit story comprehension for both groups, and (c) the children recalled significantly more story elements and the length of the story retellings was greater with the aid of animated illustrations. The findings indicate that a digital storybook provides close temporal contiguity of text and visuals and may enhance story understanding by concretizing the narration. The study provides evidence that multimedia stories can foster children’s implicit story comprehension and inferential thinking about the content of the story.

Keywords

Multimedia Electronic Printed storybooks Story comprehension Explicit Implicit 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by the Ahi Evran University Scientific Research Projects Coordination Unit. Project No. EGT.A4.18.020. Part of this study was presented at 1st International Conference on Basic Education (2018), Bursa, Turkey.

References

  1. Alberta Learning. (2000). English language arts K–9. Edmonton, AB: Alberta Education. Retrieved from https://education.alberta.ca/media/160360/ela-pos-k-9.pdf.
  2. Algozzine, B., & Douville, P. (2004). Use mental imagery across the curriculum. Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for Children and Youth, 49(1), 36–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Altun, D. (2017). Young children’s literacy habits in digital world regarding digital equality perspective. Paper presented at the 69th OMEP World Assembly and International Conference, Opatija, Croatia. Abstract retrieved from http://programme.exordo.com/omep2017/delegates/presentation/106/.
  4. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  5. Beaty, J. J., & Pratt, L. (2003). Early literacy in preschool and kindergarten. Ohio: Merrill Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  6. Bers, M. U., Flannery, L., Kazakoff, E. R., & Sullivan, A. (2014). Computational thinking and tinkering: Exploration of an early childhood robotics curriculum. Computers & Education, 72, 145–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Biancarosa, G. (2006). Reading minds: What sentence-reading rate tells us about comprehension processing (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Harvard University, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  8. Boerma, I. E., Mol, S. E., & Jolles, J. (2016). Reading pictures for story comprehension requires mental imagery skills. Frontiers in Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bus, A. G., Takacs, Z. K., & Kegel, C. A. (2015). Affordances and limitations of electronic storybooks for young children’s emergent literacy. Developmental Review, 35, 79–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chaturvedi, S. (2016). Structured approaches for exploring interpersonal relationships in natural language text (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).University of Maryland, Maryland.Google Scholar
  11. Chera, P., & Wood, C. (2003). Animated multimedia “talking books” can promote phonological awareness in children beginning to read. Learning and Instruction, 13, 33–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cohen, N. J. (2010). The impact of language development on the psychosocial and emotional development of young chidren. In S. Rvachew (Ed.), Language development and literacy (pp. 39–43). Retrieved from http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/sites/default/files/dossiers-complets/en/language-development-and-literacy.pdf.
  13. Collins, M. F. (2016). Supporting inferential thinking in preschoolers: Effects of discussion on children’s story comprehension. Early Education and Development, 27(7), 932–956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Coltheart, M. (2005). Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. In M. J. Snowling. & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading, A handbook (pp. 6–23). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cooper, L. Z. (2005). Developmentally appropriate digital environments for young children. Library Trends, 54(2), 286–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. de Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2004). The efficacy of electronic books in fostering kindergarten children’s emergent story understanding. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(4), 378–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. De Koning, B. B., & van der Schoot, M. (2013). Becoming part of the story! Refueling the interest in visualization strategies for reading comprehension. Educational Psychology Review, 25(2), 261–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dempsey, L., & Skarakis-Doyle, E. (2017). Story comprehension in pre-readers: Understanding goal structure and generating inferences when a story has competing goals. Early Child Development and Care.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2017.1408610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dombey, H. (2009). The simple view of reading. ITE English: Readings for discussion. 1–12. Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.430.2749&rep=rep1&type=pdf.
  20. Doty, D. E., Popplewell, S. R., & Byers, G. O. (2001). Interactive CD-ROM storybooks and young readers’ reading comprehension. Journal of Research on Computing in Education, 33, 374–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Engel, R. J., & Schutt, R. K. (2014). Fundamentals of social work research. Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  22. Evans, M. A., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2005). What children are looking at during shared storybook reading: Evidence from eye movement monitoring. Psychological Science, 16(11), 913–920.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01636.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, M. (2011). Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: The role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Processes, 48(2), 119–138.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2010.494244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fraenkel, J. R., Wallen, N. E., & Hyun, H. H. (2012). How to design and evaluate research in education. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  25. Giménez-Dasí, M., Pons, F., & Bender, P. K. (2016). Imaginary companions, theory of mind and emotion understanding in young children. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 24(2), 186–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gómez, F., Nussbaum, M., Weitz, J. F., Lopez, X., Mena, J., & Torres, A. (2013). Co-located single display collaborative learning for early childhood education. Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 8, 225–244.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-013-9168-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gonzales, C. R., Fabricius, W. V., & Kupfer, A. S. (2017). Introspection plays an early role in children’s explicit theory of mind development. Child Development.  https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Halliday, M. A. K. (2004). Three aspects of children’s language development: Learning language, learning through language, learning about language. In J. Webster (Ed.), The collected works of Michael Halliday: The language of early childhood (pp. 28–59). New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
  30. Hannon, B., & Frias, D. (2012). A new measure for assessing the contributions of higher level processes to language comprehension performance in preschoolers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 897–921.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Heilmann, J., Miller, J. F., Nockerts, A., & Dunaway, C. (2010). Properties of the narrative scoring scheme using narrative retells in young school-age children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(2), 154–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hood, M., Conlon, E., & Andrews, G. (2008). Preschool home literacy practices and children’s literacy development: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 252–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ihmeideh, F. M. (2014). The effect of electronic books on enhancing emergent literacy skills of pre-school children. Computers & Education, 79, 40–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Isbell, R., Sobol, J., Lindauer, L., & Lowrance, A. (2004). The effects of storytelling and story reading on the oral language complexity and story comprehension of young children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 32(3), 157–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ito, M. (2009). Engineering play: A cultural history of children’s software. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  37. John, S. F., Lui, M., & Tannock, R. (2003). Children’s story retelling and comprehension using a new narrative resource. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 18(1–2), 91–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kamil, M. L., Intrator, S. M., & Kim, H. S. (2000). The effects of other technologies on literacy and literacy learning. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal., P. D. Pearson & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research III (pp. 771–788). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  39. Kendeou, P., Van Den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. (2007). Comprehension in preschool and early elementary children: Skill development and strategy interventions. In D. S. McNamara (Ed.), Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, interventions, and technologies (pp. 27–45). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  40. Kim, Y. S. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Korat, O. (2009). The effects of CD-ROM storybook reading on children’s emergent literacy as a function of age group and repeated reading. Education and Information Technologies, 14, 39–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Korat, O. (2010). Reading electronic books as a support for vocabulary, story comprehension and word reading in kindergarten and first grade. Computers & Education, 55(1), 24–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Korat, O., & Shamir, A. (2012). Direct and indirect teaching: Using e-books for supporting vocabulary, word reading, and story comprehension for young children. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 46(2), 135–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Krcmar, M., & Cingel, D. P. (2014). Parent–child joint reading in traditional and electronic formats. Media Psychology, 17, 262–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Labbo, L. D., & Kuhn, M. R. (2000). Weaving chains of affect and cognition: A young child’s understanding of CD-ROM talking books. Journal of Literacy Research, 32, 187–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. LaRusso, M., Kim, H. Y., Selman, R., Uccelli, P., Dawson, T., Jones, S., et al. (2016). Contributions of academic language, perspective taking, and complex reasoning to deep reading comprehension. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 9(2), 201–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lauricella, A. R., Barr, R. F., & Calvert, S. L. (2014). Parent-child interactions during traditional and computer storybook reading for children’s comprehension: Implications for electronic storybook design. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 2, 17–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lepola, J., Lynch, J., Laakkonen, E., Silvén, M., & Niemi, P. (2012). The role of inference making and other language skills in the development of narrative listening comprehension in 4–6- year- old children. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(3), 259–282.  https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Lervåg, A., Hulme, C., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2017). Unpicking the developmental relationship between oral language skills and reading comprehension: It’s simple, but complex. Child Development, 58(10), 1132–1140.  https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Leu, D. J. (2000). Literacy and technology: Deictic consequences for literacy education in an information age. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 743–770). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  51. Longobardi, E., Spataro, P., & Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2016). Relations between theory of mind, mental state language and social adjustment in primary school children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(4), 424–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Malec, A., Peterson, S. S., & Elshereif, H. (2017). Assessing young children’s oral language: Recommendations for classroom practice and policy. Canadian Journal of Education, 40(3), 362–392.Google Scholar
  53. Marsh, J., Brooks, G., Hughes, J., Ritchie, L., & Roberts, S. (2005). Digital beginnings: Young children’s use of popular culture, media and new technologies. Sheffield: University of Sheffield.Google Scholar
  54. Martucci, K. (2016). Shared storybook reading in the preschool setting and considerations for young children’s theory of mind development. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 14(1), 55–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Mayer, R. E. (2005). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 31–48). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Moody, A. K., Justice, L. M., & Cabell, S. Q. (2010). Electronic versus traditional storybooks: Relative influence on preschool children’s engagement and communication. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10, 294–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Morrow, L. M. (2009). Literacy development in the early years: Helping children read and write (6th edn.). Boston: Pearson.Google Scholar
  58. Muñoz, M. L., Gillam, R. B., Peña, E. D., & Gulley-Faehnle, A. (2003). Measures of language development in fictional narratives of Latino children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 34(4), 332–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the national early literacy panel. Washington: National Institute for Literacy.Google Scholar
  60. Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 91–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Otto, B. (2006). Language development in early childhood. Upper Saddle River: Pearson.Google Scholar
  62. Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554–566.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Owens, R. E. (2012). Language development: An introduction (8th edn.). Upper Saddle River: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  64. Paivio, A. (2007). Mind and its evolution: A dual coding theoretical approach. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
  65. Parette, H. P., Quesenberry, A. C., & Blum, C. (2010). Missing the boat with technology usage in early childhood settings: A 21st century view of developmentally appropriate practice. Early Childhood Education Journal, 37, 335–343.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-009-0352-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Paris, A. H., & Paris, S. G. (2003). Assessing narrative comprehension in young children. Reading Research Quarterly, 38(1), 36–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Parish-Morris, J., Mahajan, N., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Collins, M. F. (2013). Once upon a time: Parent–child dialogue and storybook reading in the electronic era. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7(3), 200–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Pelletier, J. (2006). Relations among theory of mind, metacognitive language, reading skills and story comprehension in L1 and L2 learners. In A. Antonietti, O. Liverta-Sempio & A. Marchetti (Eds.), Theory of mind and language in developmental contexts (pp. 77–92). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Pence, K. H., & Justice, L. M. (2008). Language development from theory and practice. Upper Saddle River: Pearson.Google Scholar
  70. Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Richter, A., & Courage, M. L. (2017). Comparing electronic and paper storybooks for preschoolers: Attention, engagement, and recall. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 48, 92–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Rideout, V. (2011). Zero to eight: Children’s media use in America. San Francisco: Common Sense Media.Google Scholar
  73. Rideout, V. J. (2014). Learning at home: Families’ educational media use in America. A report of the families and media project. New York: The Joan Ganz Cooney Center.Google Scholar
  74. Roque, V. J., Teodoro, L. A., Cunanan, B. M., & Evangelista, M. T. (2017). Using animated e-storybooks to develop Filipino vocabulary and story comprehension among preschool children. Presented at De La Salle University Congress. Retrieved from http://www.dlsu.edu.ph/conferences/dlsu-research-congress-proceedings/2017/LLI/LLI-I-028.pdf.
  75. Roskos, K., Burstein, K., Shang, Y., & Gray, E. (2014). Young children’s engagement with e-books at school: Does device matter? SAGE Open, 4(1),  https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244013517244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Rudman, R., & Titjen, F. (2018). Language development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  77. Scarborough, H. S. (2009). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis) abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In F. F. Campell, J. Soler & G. Reid (Eds.), Approaching difficulties in literacy development: Assessment, pedagogy and programmes (pp. 23–38). Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  78. Schaughency, E., Suggate, S., & Reese, E. (2017). Links between early oral narrative and decoding skills and later reading in a New Zealand sample. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 22(2), 109–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Schnotz, W., & Bannert, M. (2003). Construction and interference in learning from multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 13(2), 141–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Schüler, A., Arndt, J., & Scheiter, K. (2015). Processing multimedia material: Does integration of text and pictures result in a single or two interconnected mental representations? Learning and Instruction, 35, 62–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Segers, E., Takke, L., & Veroeven, L. (2004). Teacher-mediated versus computer-mediated storybook reading to children in native and multicultural kindergarten classrooms. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 15, 215–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Smeets, D. J., & Bus, A. G. (2012). Interactive electronic storybooks for kindergartners to promote vocabulary growth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 36–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53(2), 165–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Snow, C. E. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward a research and development program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica: Office of Education Research and Improvement (OERI), Science and Technology policy institute, RAND Reading Study Group, RAND Corporation.Google Scholar
  85. Soderman, A. K., Gregory, K. M., & McCarty, L. T. (2005). Scaffolding emergent literacy (2th edn.). New York: Pearson.Google Scholar
  86. Spencer, P. E., & Koester, L. S. (2015). Nurturing language and learning: Development of deaf and hard-of-hearing infants and toddlers. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  87. Spencer, T. D., Goldstein, H., Kelley, E. S., Sherman, A., & McCune, L. (2017). A curriculum-based measure of language comprehension for preschoolers: Reliability and validity of the assessment of story comprehension. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 42(4), 209–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934–947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Strasser, K., & del Río, F. (2013). The role of comprehension monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth in predicting story comprehension and recall of kindergarten children. Reading Research Quarterly, 49(2), 169–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Strasser, K., Larraín, A., López de Lérida, S., & Lissi, M. R. (2010). La comprensión narrativa en edad preescolar: Un instrumento para su medición [Narrative comprehension in the preschool age: An assessment instrument]. Psykhe: Revista de la Escuela de Psicología, 19(1), 75–87.  https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-22282010000100006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Stuart, E. A. (2010). Matching methods for causal inference: A review and a look forward. Statistical Science: A Review Journal of the Institute of Mathematical Statistics, 25(1), 1–21.  https://doi.org/10.1214/09-STS313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Takacs, Z. K., & Bus, A. G. (2016). Benefits of motion in animated storybooks for children’s visual attention and story comprehension. An eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1591.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Takacs, Z. K., Swart, E. K., & Bus, A. G. (2014). Can the computer replace the adult for storybook reading? A meta-analysis on the effects of multimedia stories as compared to sharing print stories with an adult. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1366.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Takacs, Z. K., Swart, E. K., & Bus, A. G. (2015). Benefits and pitfalls of multimedia and interactive features in technology-enhanced storybooks: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 85(4), 698–739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Tannock, R. (2010). Language development and literacy: Comments on Beitchman and Cohen. In S. Rvachew (Ed.), Language development and literacy (pp. 44–47). Retrieved from http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/sites/default/files/dossiers-complets/en/language-development-and-literacy.pdf.
  97. Tompkins, V., Guo, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2013). Inference generation, story comprehension, and language skills in the preschool years. Reading and Writing, 26(3), 403–429.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9374-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Uccelli, P., & Páez, M. M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary development of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and associations among English and Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38(3), 225–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Uchikoshi, Y., Yang, L., & Liu, S. (2018). Role of narrative skills on reading comprehension: Spanish–English and Cantonese–English dual language learners. Reading and Writing, 31(2), 381–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO). (2013). Implementation of the United Nations Literacy Decade (2003–2012) and Specific Recommendations for the Post-Decade Period. [Executive Board 192/EX7].Retrieved from http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0022/002243/224374e.pdf.
  101. Van den Broek, P. D., Kendeou, P., Kremer, K., Lynch, J., Butler, J., White, M. J., & Lorch, E. P. (2005). Assessment of comprehension abilities in young children. In S. G. Paris & S. A. Stahl (Eds.), Children’s reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 107–130). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  102. Verhallen, M. J., & Bus, A. G. (2009). Video storybook reading as a remedy for vocabulary deficits: Outcomes and processes. Journal for Educational Research, 1(1), 172–196.Google Scholar
  103. Verhallen, M. J., & Bus, A. G. (2010). Low-income immigrant pupils learning vocabulary through digital picture storybooks. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 54–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Verhallen, M. J., Bus, A. G., & de Jong, M. T. (2006). The promise of multimedia stories for kindergarten children at risk. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 410–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Wang, S. X., Wang, Z. H., Cheng, X. T., Li, J., Sang, Z. P., Zhang, X. D., & Wang, Z. Q. (2007). Arsenic and fluoride exposure in drinking water: Children’s IQ and growth in Shanyin county, Shanxi province, China. Environmental Health Perspectives, 115(4), 643–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Wermke, K., Leising, D., & Stellzig-Eisenhauer, A. (2007). Relation of melody complexity in infants’ cries to language outcome in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(11–12), 961–973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Willoughby, D., Evans, M. A., & Nowak, S. (2015). Do ABC e-books boost engagement and learning in preschoolers? An experimental study comparing e-books with paper ABC and storybook controls. Computers & Education, 82, 107–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Yang, T. C., Hwang, G. J., Yang, S. J., & Hwang, G. H. (2015). A two-tier test-based approach to Improving students’ computer-programming skills in a web-based learning environment. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 18(1), 198–210.Google Scholar
  109. Yang, T. C., & Wu, W. C. (2012). Digital storytelling for enhancing student academic achievement, critical thinking, and learning motivation: A year-long experimental study. Computers& Education, 59(2), 339–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Zucker, T. A., Moody, A. K., & McKenna, M. C. (2009). The effects of electronic books on pre-kindergarten-to-grade 5 students’ literacy and language outcomes: A search synthesis. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 40(1), 47–87.  https://doi.org/10.2190/EC.40.1.c.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Children’s Literature Cited

  1. Lionni, L. (2013). Yüzyüz [Swimmy]. Ankara: Elma Yayınları.Google Scholar
  2. Oral, F. (2008). Kırmızı elma [The red apple]. Ankara: Yapı Kredi Yayınları.Google Scholar
  3. Would. H. S. (2016). Ay’ı kim çaldı? [Who stole the moon]. Ankara: Yapı Kredi Yayınları.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Early Childhood EducationAhi Evran UniversityKırşehirTurkey

Personalised recommendations