As Table 2 shows, in the comparison between countries, we found a statistically significant difference in all the factors. However, many factors have a very similar trend. This is crucial because it indicates that students from the four countries had similar perceptions and experiences during the transition to emergency online learning. First, we present the results of the factors during the transition to emergency online learning due to COVID-19. Then, we present a model explaining how these factors may predict cognitive engagement during online learning among the USA, Mexico, Peru, and Turkey.
Table 2 Educational technology acceptance factors based on Kemp et al.’s (2019) taxonomy Attitude, affect, and motivation
For attitude, affect, and motivation, we asked specific questions to each sub-group.
Attitude
Attitude refers to the individual’s positive or negative evaluation of the behavior (Kemp et al. 2019; Venkatesh et al. 2003). The students’ preference for a teaching/learning delivery method implies a positive attitude towards the method. Therefore, we asked students about their preference towards face-to-face or remote teaching, and if they struggled with adapting to remote learning and teaching. In Mexico, Peru, Turkey, and the USA, participants showed a strong preference for the face-to-face learning method over online learning.
The more students preferred the face to face method, the more they struggled during the emergency online learning. The four countries showed a significant positive moderate correlation between preference for face-to-face and struggling with adapting to online learning: Mexico, rs(323) = .46, p < .001; Peru, rs(299) = .45, p < .001; Turkey, rs(125) = .37, p < .001; and the USA, rs(249) = .54, p < .001. Also, in the four countries, there was a negative significant correlation between preference for online learning and struggle (Mexico, rs(323) = −.38, p < .001; Peru, rs(299) = −.50, p < .001; Turkey, rs(125) = −.23, p < .05; and USA rs(249) = −45, p < .001,) indicating fewer students struggled in adapting to the emergency online learning.
Moreover, the preference of students towards face-to-face learning negatively impacted their cognitive engagement, especially in the USA rs(234) = −.39, p < .001 and Peru rs(299) = −.39, p < .001, where there was a significantly negative moderate relationship. Students from Mexico rs(323) = −.17 and Turkey rs(125) = −.19, showed a weak negative relationship at the p < .05 level. On the other hand, there was a positive significant relationship between students’ preference for online learning and students’ cognitive engagement. The USA, rs(234) = .38, and Peru rs(292) = .47 showed a moderate correlation, and Mexico rs(323) = .30 a weak correlation, all at the p < .001 level. Turkey showed a weak correlation, rs(125) = .23 at the p < .05 level. Responses from the four countries confirmed that students who preferred online learning had a better cognitive engagement and struggled less during the emergency online learning than students who preferred face-to-face (negative attitude towards online learning). Moreover, participants who preferred face-to-face learning had more difficulties during the emergency online learning period and had a negative cognitive engagement in the four countries.
Affect
Affect refers to the user’s satisfaction with the use of technology and the user’s emotional state (Guerrero 2019; Saadé and Bahli 2005). We asked students if they were satisfied with their courses. There was a statistically significant difference between the four countries. Students from Mexico showed more satisfaction with courses, followed by the USA, Peru, and Turkey. Students from the four countries showed a positive correlation between satisfaction with their courses and cognitive engagement (Mexico, rs(323) = .35, Peru rs(299) = .52, Turkey rs(125) = .50, and USA rs(235) = .48, all at the p < .001 level).
Regarding the students’ emotional state, students were asked: “Describe how your emotional states have changed after the stay-at-home order related to COVID-19”, and we listed life satisfaction and happiness for wellbeing. The construct showed a good internal consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha for the USA α = .84, Mexico α = .88, Peru α = .86 and Turkey α = .86). For negative emotions, we listed stress, anxiety, and apathy with good internal consistency in the four countries (Cronbach’s Alpha for the USA α = .78, Mexico α = .87, Peru α = .88, and Turkey α = .88).
Students’ emotional states were compared between countries using a one-way ANOVA test that showed significant differences for wellbeing (F = 32.48, p < .001) and negative emotions (F = 7.137, p < .001). Post hoc comparison using Tukey HSD for wellbeing showed that only Mexican students’ wellbeing was statistically higher than students’ wellbeing in the other three countries. The USA, Turkey, and Peru students’ responses did not vary significantly. For negative emotions, post hoc Tukey comparison showed that students from the four countries experienced an increase in their negative emotions during the stay-at-home order without significant difference between the USA, Mexico, Peru, and Turkey.
Motivation
Motivation refers to the students’ drive to learn (Maldonado et al. 2009; Jung and Lee 2020). For this factor, we asked about the students’ reasons for pursuing school before and after the transition. The reasons listed were: talk to classmates, interact with professors, hang out, do academic activities, complete projects, interest in in-class topics, and finish degree studies. We calculated a mean score for the seven “before” and “after” items for each country. Using Cronbach’s Alpha analysis, responses showed a good internal consistency for the four countries in both scenarios (USA α1 = 0.83, α2 = 0.87; Mexico α1 = .80, α2 = .89; Peru α1 = 0.73, α2 = 0.79; Turkey α1 = 0.83, α2 = 0.85).
When comparing motivation before and after, students’ motivation from Mexico t(323) = 4.57, p < .001, Peru, t(284) = 14.27, p < .001, and the USA t(239) = 13.14, p < .001, decreased after the transition to online learning, while Turkish students’ motivation did not change significantly (t(125) = 1.37, p > .05). We also examined the relation between students’ motivation after the transition and their cognitive engagement. Students showed a positive significant correlation, being moderate in the USA, r(225) = .35 and weak in Peru, r(283) = .28, both at the p < .001 level. Mexico showed a weak positive correlation, r(225) = .16 at the p < .05 level, and Turkey did not show a significant correlation between motivation after transition and cognitive engagement. This means that in the USA, Peru, and Mexico, the lower the students’ motivation during online learning, the worse their cognitive engagement.
Perceived behavioral control
Use of technology
Regarding the use of technology, we asked students how frequently they used it before and after the transition to online learning. The use of technology before refers to students’ previous experiences with technology that facilitate the ease of use (Abdullah and Ward 2016; Kemp et al. 2019; Venkatesh et al. 2003). The questions were the same for before and after the transition to online learning. We asked students, “For educational purposes, how often did you use: an online educational platform such as Canvas or Blackboard; communication tools such as Zoom or Teams, social media such as LinkedIn, Facebook, etc.; asynchronous videos assigned or typed by instructors; and synchronous class sessions?”
A one-way ANOVA and post hoc Tukey HSD analysis showed statistically significant differences in the use of technology before the transition among the four countries (F = 85.72, p < .001). The USA students had more experience with technology, followed by Mexico, Turkey, and lastly Peru.
We compared the use of technology before and after the stay-at-home order, and all the participants significantly increased their use of technology during the stay-at-home order (USA t(238) = 19.02, p < .001; Mexico t(323) = 41.06, p < .001; Peru t(289) = 33.74, p < .001; and Turkey t(125) = 21.85, p < .001). The sudden transition to emergency online learning forced students from the four countries to become more aware of technological tools than they previously were.
In the USA there is also a weak correlation between previous use of technology and preference for online learning (r(242) = .20, p < .05). This correlation was not significant for Mexico, Peru, or Turkey. US Students who preferred online learning had more experience with technology.
Furthermore, we found a significant positive weak correlation between previous use of technology (ease of use) and self-efficacy in the USA, r(236) = .15, p < .05; Mexico, r(323) = .17, p < .05; Peru r(283) = .15, p < .05; and Turkey r(125) = .28, p < .05. Data shows that the more experience students have with technology, the higher their perception of their own capabilities, in the four countries.
Self-efficacy
Self-efficacy refers to the students’ judgment of their capabilities (Bandura 1977, 1986; Guerrero 2019). Students were asked how their following abilities have changed since the stay-at-home order: ability to complete assignments on time, ability to be successful in classes, ability to discuss topics with classmates and/or professors, and time management skills. The Cronbach’s alpha analysis shows a good internal consistency in the four countries (USA α = .86, Mexico α = .83, Peru α = .79, Turkey α = 0.80).
A one-way ANOVA analysis and post hoc comparisons using the Tukey HSD test indicated that students from Peru, Mexico, and Turkey showed a similar perception of self-efficacy (Table 2). Only students from the USA showed a statistically significant difference as compared to the other three countries. Even though the US students had more experience with technology, and there was a positive weak correlation between use of technology before transition and self-efficacy (r(236) = .15, p < .05), they showed the lowest mean in self-efficacy.
Accessibility
Accessibility refers to the degree to which a student perceives that access to educational technology is present (Kemp et al. 2019; Pham and Tran 2020). For this factor, we asked how often students have access to a reliable digital device (computer, tablet, or mobile device), a reliable internet service, communication software such as Skype, Zoom, or Teams, and if they have access to solve technical issues. Using the Cronbach’s alpha test, responses showed an acceptable internal consistency in the USA (α = .77), Mexico (α = .72), and Turkey (α = .80), but not in Peru (α = .61).
A one-way ANOVA analysis and post hoc comparison of means (Tukey HSD) showed a significant difference where Mexican participants have more accessibility than students from the USA, Turkey, and Peru. The USA, Peru, and Turkey had similar means for accessibility.
Cognitive engagement
Cognitive engagement refers to the focus, attention, and absorption of the learner (Kemp et al. 2019; Saadé and Bahli 2005). Students were asked to report changes in their academic performance compared to how they were before the COVID-19 stay-at-home order. We listed six categories: grades, knowledge/learning (related to school), concentration, level of engagement, class attendance, and interest and enthusiasm. We calculated Cronbach’s alpha, which yielded a positive internal consistency in all four countries (USA α = .92, Mexico α = .87, Peru α = .88, Turkey α = 0.88).
There was a statistically significant difference between the countries when compared to a one-way ANOVA test. Post hoc comparison using the Tukey HSD test indicated that US students had a statistically significant lower cognitive engagement than students from Mexico, Peru, and Turkey.
Cognitive engagement is usually lower in the online environment than in face-to-face classes. It is important to keep students engaged to have positive learning outcomes (Panigrahi et al. 2018). This research presents a model with the factors that may predict the use of online learning in the future based on the students’ experience during emergency online learning.
Even though we followed Kemp et al.’s (2019) taxonomy with the most common factors to predict the use and acceptance of technology, we included only factors that may influence the students’ cognitive engagement while using online learning in the future. For example, for attitude, we did not include face-to-face preference, only preference for online learning. For affect, we did not include emotional states because we assumed that they may not be completely reliable due to the pandemic; however, we included satisfaction with courses. We also did not include motivation after the stay-at-home order because we assumed that motivation before would be the regular motivation of each student for pursuing studies. The model and its results are as follows (Fig. 2).
A linear model showed statistically significant differences between factors among the four countries. In the USA (r2 = 0.47, F = 29.35, p < .001), this model explains 47% of the variability for cognitive engagement but only self-efficacy (β = .67, p < .001) and preference for online learning (β = .67, p < .05) showed significance towards cognitive engagement. This indicates that attitude and self-efficacy are relevant factors for predicting cognitive engagement among students in the USA while using online learning.
Regarding Mexico (r2 = .34, F = 27.76, p < .001), the multivariate model explained the 34% of variability. There were four significant factors that predict cognitive engagement among Mexican students, self-efficacy (β = .454, p < .001), preference for online learning (β = .115, p < .05), motivation to pursue studies before the transition (β = .169, p < .05), and accessibility (β = .237, p < .05). For Peru, the model explains 61% of the variability (r2 = .59, F = 61.34, p < .001). The factors that were statistically significant were self-efficacy (β = .498, p < .001), preference for online learning (β = .169, p < .05), and satisfaction with courses (β = .169, p < .05). Finally, for Turkey (r2 = .491, F = 20.91, p < .001), the model explains 49% of the variability and only self-efficacy (β = .601, p < .001) was significant enough to predict cognitive engagement.
Self-efficacy is the only factor that predicts cognitive engagement in online learning for the four countries. However, each country shows different significant factors that predict cognitive engagement among students.