Abstract
Background
Little is known about how self-schemas are formed, fluctuate, and are reinforced. In this study, we used a revised mnemic neglect paradigm to examine how self-schema fluctuates following episodic events (feedback) and its self-concordance.
Methods
Participants exhibiting various depressive symptoms (BDI-II ranging from 1 to 36; M = 11.90) underwent psychological testing, followed by bogus feedback regarding their personality, future, and behavioural traits, where they rated their state self-schemas and feedback self-concordance trial-by-trial.
Results and Conclusions
Linear mixed models showed that feedback self-concordance was determined by the interaction between self-schema and the emotional valence of the feedback, and the self-schema fluctuated with the interaction between prediction error (the difference between the emotional valence of the feedback and current self-schema) and feedback self-concordance. Cognitive reactivity, the ease of responding to negative moods, was associated with higher parameters regressed onto self-schema and self-concordance regardless of the feedback valence, indicating that it enhances the likelihood of self-schema fluctuation positively and negatively. The simulation of self-schema development shows that some individuals developed a negative self-schema even after experiencing many positive events; these parameters were characteristic of individuals with high levels of cognitive reactivity. These results have significant implications for self-schema development and depression.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


Notes
A list of feedback items in Japanese is available from the first author.
We were also interested in whether the amount of change in the self-schema and cognitive reactivity predicted memory performance on a per-feedback basis. Unfortunately, we were unable to conduct such an analysis due to the loss of 20 participants’ memory recording forms. The data of these memory recollections under each emotional valence were available because they were tabulated before being lost.
A significant interaction among the emotional valence of feedback, feedback self-concordance and LEIDS-R was observed in the three-way mixed effect model for the current self-schema after controlling for the state self-schema in the pretrial period (γ = − 0.005, t = − 3.58, p < .001). The two-way interaction between the emotional valence of the feedback and feedback self-concordance was also significant (γ = − 0.13, t = − 5.51, p < .001). Under the positive feedback conditions, high self-concordance predicted positive changes in the self-schema (γ = 0.06, t = 3.22, p = .001), and under the negative feedback conditions, high self-concordance predicted negative changes in the self-schema (γ = − 0.06, t = − 4.06, p < .001). Here, LEIDS-R further strengthened the interaction between the emotional valence of feedback and feedback self-concordance.
Although this note is speculative, the simulations conducted in this study may also explain the pathology of narcissistic personality (Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010). The exploratory correlational analysis results show that narcissistic personality scores are associated with α. Narcissistic personality disorder is characterized by a strong desire to receive approval and admiration from others (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), which strengthens positive self-schema and feelings of grandiosity. Individuals with this disorder also experience emotional vulnerability when experiencing criticism and frustration. In the simulations involving high α and low w values with 20% and 50% negative events, the self-schema oscillated between positive and negative, which may reflect the emotional vulnerability experienced by those with narcissistic personality disorder. However, we observed several cases of persistent exaggeration in which both α and w were high. Once the tendency for positive self-schema forms, the process of selectively noticing positive information may be a pathology in narcissistic personalities.
References
Alba, J., Calvete, E., Wante, L., Van Beveren, M.-L., & Braet, C. (2018). Early maladaptive schemas as moderators of the association between bullying victimization and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42, 24–35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9874-5
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Author.
Antypa, N., & Van der Does, A. J. W. (2010). Serotonin transporter gene, childhood emotional abuse and cognitive vulnerability to depression. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 9, 615–620. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00593.x
Barnhofer, T., & Chittka, T. (2010). Cognitive reactivity mediates the relationship between neuroticism and depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 275–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.12.005
Barsalou, L. W., Huttenlocher, J., & Lamberts, K. (1998). Basing categorization on individuals and events. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 203–272. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0687
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge University Press.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory–II. Psychological Corporation.
Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavior therapy (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 300–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x
Booij, L., & Van der Does, A. J. W. (2007). Cognitive and serotonergic vulnerability to depression: Convergent findings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 86–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.86
Bowles, D. P., Armitage, C. J., Drabble, J., & Meyer, B. (2013). Self-esteem and other-esteem in college students with borderline and avoidant personality disorder features: An experimental vignette study. Personality and Mental Health, 7, 307–319. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1230
Bowman, C. R., & Zeithamova, D. (2018). Abstract memory representations in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus support concept generalization. Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 2605–2614. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2811-17.2018
Bruce, A. E., Cole, D. A., Dallaire, D. H., Jacquez, F. M., Pineda, A. Q., & LaGrange, B. (2006). Relations of parenting and negative life events to cognitive diatheses for depression in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 321–333. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-006-9019-x
Brysbaert, M., & Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of Cognition, 1, 9. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.10
Cyders, M. A., Littlefield, A. K., Coffey, S., & Karyadi, K. A. (2014). Examination of a short English version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale. Addictive Behaviors, 39, 1372–1376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.02.013
Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Haigh, E. A., & Beck, A. T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 467–477. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3027
Van der Does, A. J. W., & Williams, J. M. G. (2003). Leiden Index of Depression Sensitivity-Revised (LEIDS-R). Leiden University.
Van der Does, A. J. W., Barnhofer, T., & Williams, J. M. G. (2003). The Major Depression Questionnaire (MDQ). www.dousa.nl/publications.
Dozois, D. J. A., & Beck, A. T. (2008). Cognitive schemas, beliefs and assumptions. In K. S. Dobson & D. J. A. Dozois (Eds.), Risk Factors in Depression (pp. 121–143). Elsevier/Academic Press.
Dozois, D. J. A., & Rnic, K. (2015). Core beliefs and self-schematic structure in depression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 4, 98–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.008
Eldar, E., & Niv, Y. (2015). Interaction between emotional state and learning underlies mood instability. Nature Communications, 6, 6149. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms7149
Farb, N. A. S., Irving, J. A., Anderson, A. K., & Segal, Z. V. (2015). A two-factor model of relapse/recurrence vulnerability in unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 38–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000031
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997). User’s guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). American Psychiatric Press.
Fowler, D., Freeman, D., Smith, B., Kuipers, E., Bebbington, P., Bashforth, H., Coker, S., Hodgekins, J., Gracie, A., Dunn, G., & Garety, P. (2006). The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS): Psychometric properties and associations with paranoia and grandiosity in non-clinical and psychosis samples. Psychological Medicine, 36(6), 749–759. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706007355
Gilboa, A., & Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 618–631. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.013
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 1022–1038. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1987). A parallel form of the Gudjonsson suggestibility scale. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 215–221. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1987.tb01348.x
Huys, Q. J., Pizzagalli, D. A., Bogdan, R., & Dayan, P. (2013). Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: A behavioural meta-analysis. Biology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders, 3(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-3-12
Iijima, Y., Takano, K., Boddez, Y., Raes, F., & Tanno, Y. (2017). Stuttering thoughts: Negative self-referent thinking is less sensitive to aversive outcomes in people with higher levels of depressive symptoms. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1333. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01333
Irish, M., & Piguet, O. (2013). The pivotal role of semantic memory in remembering the past and imagining the future. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00027
Korn, C. W., Prehn, K., Park, S. Q., Walter, H., & Heekeren, H. R. (2012). Positively biased processing of self-relevant social feedback. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(47), 16832–16844. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3016-12.2012
Kube, T., & Rozenkrantz, L. (2020). When beliefs face reality: An integrative review of belief updating in mental health and illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620931496
Leahy, R. L. (2015). Emotional schema therapy. Guilford Press.
Liu, R. T., & Alloy, L. B. (2010). Stress generation in depression: A systematic review of the empirical literature and recommendations for future study. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 582–593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.010
Lumley, M. N., & Harkness, K. L. (2007). Specificity in the relations among childhood adversity, early maladaptive schemas, and symptom profiles in adolescent depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 639–657. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9100-3
Lynam, D. R., Smith, G. T., Whiteside, S. P., & Cyders, M. A. (2006). The UPPS-P: Assessing five personality pathways to impulsive behavior (Technical Report). Purdue University.
Meehl, P. E. (1956). Wanted—A good cook-book. American Psychologist, 11, 263–272. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044164
Mezulis, A. H., Hyde, J. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (2006). The developmental origins of cognitive vulnerability to depression: Temperament, parenting, and negative life events in childhood as contributors to negative cognitive style. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1012–1025. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1012
Miranda, J., & Persons, J. B. (1988). Dysfunctional attitudes are mood-state dependent. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(1), 76–79. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.97.1.76
Moulds, M. L., Kandris, E., Williams, A. D., Lang, T., Yap, C., & Hoffmeister, K. (2008). An investigation of the relationship between cognitive reactivity and rumination. Behavior Therapy, 39, 65–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2007.05.001
Naka, M. (1998). False memories and some scales: GSS, CIS, and DES [In Japanese]. Bulletin of the Faculty of Education, Chiba University, 46, 1–18.
Nørby, S. (2018). Forgetting and emotion regulation in mental health, anxiety and depression. Memory, 26, 342–363. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1346130
Ojanen, T., & Perry, D. G. (2007). Relational schemas and the developing self: Perceptions of mother and of self as joint predictors of early adolescents’ self-esteem. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1474–1483. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1474
Pincus, A. L., & Lukowitsky, M. R. (2010). Pathological narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 421–446. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131215
Preston, A. R., & Eichenbaum, H. (2013). Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Current Biology, 23, R764–R773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041
Raes, F., Ghesquière, I., & Van Gucht, D. (2012). Cognitive reactivity to success and failure relate uniquely to manic and depression tendencies and combine in bipolar tendencies. Depression Research and Treatment. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/753946
Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R. T., & du Toit, M. (2011). HLM 7: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Scientific Software International.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton University Press.
Saunders, J. (2011). Reversed mnemic neglect of self-threatening memories in dysphoria. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 854–867. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.524037
Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (2000). On the self-protective nature of inconsistency-negativity management: Using the person memory paradigm to examine self-referent memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 906–922. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.906
Sedikides, C., Green, J. D., Saunders, J., Skowronski, J. J., & Zengel, B. (2016). Mnemic neglect: Selective amnesia of one’s faults. European Review of Social Psychology, 27, 1–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2016.1183913
Segal, Z. V., Kennedy, S., Gemar, M., Hood, K., Pedersen, R., & Buis, T. (2006). Cognitive reactivity to sad mood provocation and the prediction of depressive relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 749–755. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.749
Shimizu, H. (2016). An introduction to the statistical free software HAD: Suggestions to improve teaching, learning and practice data analysis [In Japanese]. Journal of Media, Information and Communication, 1, 59–73.
Smeets, T., Jelicic, J. L. M., & Merckelbach, H. (2009). Shortened versions of the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale meet the standards. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14, 149–155. https://doi.org/10.1348/135532507X190207
Squire, L. R., Genzel, L., Wixted, J. T., & Morris, R. G. (2015). Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, 7, a021766. https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021766
Takano, K., Van Grieken, J., & Raes, F. (2019). Difficulty in updating positive beliefs about negative cognition is associated with increased depressed mood. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 64, 22–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.02.001
Tariq, A., Reid, C., & Chan, S. (2021). A meta-analysis of the relationship between early maladaptive schemas and depression in adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001458
Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938808410927
Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Emotional processing, three modes of mind and the prevention of relapse in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(S1), S53–S77. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00050-9
Tse, D., Langston, R. F., Kakeyama, M., Bethus, I., Spooner, P. A., Wood, E. R., Witter, M. P., & Morris, R. G. M. (2007). Schemas and memory consolidation. Science, 316, 76–82. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1135935
Van der Does, A. J. W. (2002). Cognitive reactivity to a sad mood: Structure and validity of a new measure. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00111-X
van Kesteren, M. T., Beul, S. F., Takashima, A., Henson, R. N., Ruiter, D. J., & Fernández, G. (2013). Differential roles for medial prefrontal and medial temporal cortices in schema-dependent encoding: From congruent to incongruent. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2352–2359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.027
van Kesteren, M. T. R., Fernández, G., Norris, D. G., & Hermans, E. J. (2010). Persistent schema-dependent hippocampal–neocortical connectivity during memory encoding and post encoding rest in humans. PNAS, 107, 7550–7555. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914892107
Wenze, S. J., Gunthert, K. C., & Forand, N. R. (2010). Cognitive reactivity in everyday life as a prospective predictor of depressive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34, 554–562. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-010-9299-x
Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy: A practitioner’s guide. Guilford Press.
Funding
This work was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Grant Nos. 17J04080, 18K13344).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Ethical Approval
All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed Consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Matsumoto, N., Katahira, K. & Kawaguchi, J. Cognitive Reactivity Amplifies the Activation and Development of Negative Self-schema: A Revised Mnemic Neglect Paradigm and Computational Modelling. Cogn Ther Res 47, 38–51 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-022-10332-x
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-022-10332-x
Keywords
- Depression
- Cognitive reactivity
- Self-schema
- Mnemic neglect
- Reinforcement learning