Advertisement

Depressive Suppression: Effects of Emotion Suppression on Multiple Emotions for Depressed Versus Nondepressed Individuals

  • Matthew BolandEmail author
  • Anthony Papa
  • Elysia Oliver
  • Robert del Carlo
Original Article

Abstract

Clinical theory from multiple psychological perspectives, including cognitive therapies, have long suggested that the maladaptive use of strategies to regulate emotion play a central role in the development and maintenance of psychopathology. This consideration may be particularly salient for depressive symptoms, given the prevalence of multiple negative emotions in the context of depression and the recently established tendency for depressed individuals to suppress emotions. We experimentally tested whether emotional and physiological effects of emotion suppression in response to separate sadness and anxiety inductions are moderated by depressive symptoms. Results indicated no effect of suppression on self-reported sadness compared to control condition for depressed or nondepressed participants (total N = 113). However, suppressing anxiety led to a reduction in self-reported anxiety compared to control condition for nondepressed participants, but there was no effect of suppression on anxiety among depressed participants. A similar pattern emerged for cardiac responding during both inductions. Results suggest that the effects of suppression depend on the emotion suppressed and on one’s level of depressive symptoms. Depressed participants’ difficulty mitigating state anxiety also suggests that depressed individuals have difficulty regulating certain emotions, but not others. Further implications for theoretical understanding, assessment of psychopathology, and clinical practice are discussed.

Keywords

Emotion suppression Emotion regulation Depression Depressed affect State anxiety Cardiac responding 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Matthew Boland, Anthony Papa, Elysia Oliver, and Robert del Carlo declare that they have no conflict of interest. The authors received no funding from an external source or granting agency for this study. The first author received a small internal department grant that was used to offer optional financial compensation to participants (as discussed in Methods section of the study manuscript) and to purchase electrodes for heart rate measurements. This small grant was not used for other purposes for this study.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed involving human participants in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Animal Rights

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

References

  1. Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: Capturing context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 155–172.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459518.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology: A meta analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Allen, L. B., McHugh, R. K., & Barlow, D. H. (2008). Emotional disorders: A unified protocol. In D. H. Barlow & D. H. Barlow (Eds.), Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual (4th ed., pp. 216–249). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  4. Beck, A. T., & Emery, G. (2005). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. Cambridge: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  5. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  6. Beevers, C. G. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to depression: A dual process model. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(7), 975–1002.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2005.03.003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beevers, C. G., Clasen, P. C., Enock, P. M., & Schyder, D. M. (2015). Attention bias modification for major depressive disorder: Effects on attention bias, resting state connectivity, and symptom change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 463–475.  https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000049.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Berntson, G. G., & Stowell, J. R. (1998). ECG artifacts and heart period variability: Don’t miss a beat! Psychophysiology, 35(1), 127–132.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3510127.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Boland, M., Papa, A., & del Carlo, R. E. (2019). Trait negative affect moderates the effects of expressive versus experiential emotion suppression. Personality and Individual Differences, 138, 4–10.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.08.021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(6), 591–612.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613504116.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15, 482–487.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.09567976.2004.00705.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Burklund, L. J., Craske, M. G., Taylor, S. E., & Lieberman, M. D. (2015). Altered emotion regulation capacity in social phobia as a function of comorbidity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(2), 199–208.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu058.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3(1), 48–67.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bylsma, L. M., Morris, B. H., & Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 676–691.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006a). Acceptability and suppression of negative emotion in anxiety and mood disorders. Emotion, 4, 587–595.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.4.587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006b). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1251–1263.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Carver, C., & Connor-Smith, J. (2010). Personality and coping. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 679–704.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100352.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., Freeman, P., & Walker, J. R. (2001). Anxiety sensitivity and major depression: Examination of affective state dependence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1349–1356.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00106-6.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dennis, T. A. (2007). Interactions between emotion regulation strategies and affective style: Implications for trait anxiety versus depressed mood. Motivation and Emotion, 31(3), 200–207.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9069-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Aldao, A., & De Los Reyes, A. (2015). Emotion regulation in context: Examining the spontaneous use of strategies across emotional intensity and type of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 271–276.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dunn, B. D., Billotti, D., Murphy, V., & Dalgleish, T. (2009). The consequences of effortful emotion regulation when processing distressing material: A comparison of suppression and acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(9), 761–773.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.007.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124–129.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030377.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Freud, A. (1946). The ego and the mechanisms of defense. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  24. Friedman, H. S., & Booth-Kewley, S. (1987). Personality, type A behavior, and coronary heart disease: The role of emotional expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 783–792.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.783.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gotlib, I. H., Kasch, K. L., Traill, S., Joormann, J., Arnow, B. A., et al. (2004). Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 386–398.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.386.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gray, J. A. & McNaughton, N. (Eds.). (2003). Ethology and anxiety. In The neuropsychology of anxiety (2nd ed.). Oxford: The Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gross, J. J., & Feldman-Barrett, L. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3(1), 8–16.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910380974.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion, 9(1), 87–108.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939508408966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95–103.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.95.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., et al. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. Psychological Record, 54, 553–578.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  34. Hofmann, S. G., Heering, S., Sawyer, A. T., & Asnaani, A. (2009). How to handle anxiety: The effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal. Behaviour Research and Therapy.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Joormann, J., Waugh, C. E., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Cognitive bias modification for interpretation in major depression. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(1), 126–139.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614560748.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W., Demsler, O., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 617–627.  https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1991). Stress and immune function in humans. In R. Ader, D. L. Felten & N. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology (2nd ed., pp. 849–867). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.446.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. M. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  40. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  41. Levenson, R. W. (1999). The intrapersonal functions of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 481–504.  https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Levitt, J. T., Brown, T. A., Orsillo, S. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2004). The effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective and psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior Therapy, 35, 747–766.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(04)80018-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Liverant, G. I., Brown, T. A., Barlow, D. H., & Roemer, L. (2008). Emotion regulation in unipolar depression: The effects of acceptance and suppression of subjective emotional experience on the intensity and duration of sadness and negative affect. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(11), 1201–1209.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.08.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Llewelyn, N., Dolcos, S., Iordan, A. D., Rudolph, K. D., & Dolcos, F. (2013). Reappraisal and suppression mediate the contribution of regulatory focus to anxiety in healthy adults. Emotion, 13(4), 610–615.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mauss, I. B., Evers, C., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2006). How to bite your tongue without blowing your top: Implicit evaluation of emotion regulation predicts affective responding to anger provocation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 589–602.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205283841.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mauss, I. B., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Emotion suppression and cardiovascular disease: Is hiding your feelings bad for your heart? In L. R. Temoshok, A. Vingerhoets & I. Nyklicek (Eds.), The expression of emotion and health (pp. 62–81). London: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
  47. Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and autonomic physiology. Emotion, 5, 175–190.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.175.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mauss, I. B., Tamir, M., Anderson, C. L., & Savino, N. S. (2011). Can seeking happiness make people unhappy? Paradoxical effects of valuing happiness. Emotion, 11(4), 807–815.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022010.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mennin, D. S., Holaway, R. M., Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2007). Delineating components of emotion and its dysregulation in anxiety and mood psychopathology. Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 284–302.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.09.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Miller, J. L., Vaillancourt, T., & Hanna, S. E. (2009). The measurement of ‘eating-disorder-thoughts’ and ‘eating-disorder-behaviors’: Implications for assessment and detection of eating disorders in epidemiological studies. Eating Behaviors, 10(2), 89–96.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.02.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Moore, S. A., Zoellner, L. A., & Mollenholt, N. (2008). Are expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms? Behavior Research and Therapy, 46(9), 993–1000.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Mowrer, O. H. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553–564.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Nyklícek, I., Thayer, J. F., & Van Doornen, L. J. P. (1997). Cardiorespiratory differentiation of musically-induced emotions. Journal of Psychophysiology, 11, 304–321.Google Scholar
  55. Pignatiello, M., Camp, C. J., Elder, S. T., & Rasar, L. A. (1989). A psychophysiological comparison of the velten and musical mood induction techniques. Journal of Music Therapy, 26(3), 140–154.  https://doi.org/10.1093/jmt/26.3.140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Maiti, A. K. (1994). Vagal tone and the physiological regulation of emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 167–186.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1166144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 410–424.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.410.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Roter, D. L., & Ewart, C. K. (1992). Emotional inhibition in essential hypertension: Obstacle to communication during medical visits? Health Psychology, 11, 163–169.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.11.3.163.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Rottenberg, J. (2005). Mood and emotion in major depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 167–170.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00354.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 627–639.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.627.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. R., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 9–28). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Seeley, S. H., Mennin, D. S., Aldao, A., McLaughlin, K. A., Rottenberg, J., & Fresco, D. M. (2016). Impact of depressive disorders on subjective and physiological responses to emotion in generalized anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(3), 290–303.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9744-y.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Segerstrom, S. C., & Nes, L. S. (2007). Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychological Science, 18(3), 275–281.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01888.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Shallcross, A. J., Ford, B. Q., Floerke, V. A., & Mauss, I. B. (2013). Getting better with age: The relationship between age, acceptance, and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 734–749.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031180.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Shallcross, A. J., Troy, A., Boland, M., & Mauss, I. B. (2010). Let it be: Accepting negative emotional experiences predicts decreased negative affect and depressive symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(9), 921–929.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010. 05.025.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1391–1396.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611418350.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Tabachinik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2012). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). New York: Pearson.Google Scholar
  68. Troy, A. S., Wilhelm, F. H., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B. (2010). Seeing the silver lining: Cognitive reappraisal ability moderates the relationship between stress and depressive symptoms. Emotion, 10(6), 783–795.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020262.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Tull, M. T., & Gratz, K. L. (2008). Further examination of the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and depression: The mediating role of experiential avoidance and difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior when distressed. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(2), 199–210.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.03.005.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Tull, M. T., Gratz, K. L., & Lacroce, D. M. (2006). The role of anxiety sensitivity and lack of emotional approach coping in depressive symptom severity among a non-clinical sample of uncued panickers. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 35, 74–87.  https://doi.org/10.1080/16506070500466733.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Webb, W. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 775–808.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027600.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of California, San Diego, School of MedicineLa JollaUSA
  2. 2.University of Nevada, RenoRenoUSA
  3. 3.Oregon State University, CascadesBendUSA
  4. 4.University of Nevada School of MedicineRenoUSA

Personalised recommendations