Abstract
Bipolar disorder (BD) and major depressive disorder (MDD) cannot be reliably differentiated by depression symptom expression alone, suggesting a need to identify processes that may more effectively differentiate the two disorders. To explore this question, currently depressed adults with BD (n = 30) and MDD (n = 30), and healthy control participants with no history of psychiatric illness (CTL; n = 30), completed self-report measures of reward and punishment sensitivity (i.e., behavioral activation and inhibition) and emotion regulation processes (i.e., rumination and avoidance). Results revealed that constructs putatively linked to depression across the mood disorders (i.e., behavioral inhibition, negative rumination, dampening of positive affect, behavioral and experiential avoidance) were significantly higher in both mood disorder groups compared to CTLs. Yet there was also some specificity between mood disorder groups, such that the BD group reported significantly greater reward responsiveness and positive rumination, in addition to greater behavioral inhibition and avoidance, compared to the MDD group. These data suggest that patterns of affective responding previously linked to underlying risk for mania in BD may remain evident during a major depressive episode. Further, current models of reward sensitivity in BD may benefit from the inclusion of punishment sensitivity and behavioral avoidance, particularly with respect to bipolar depression.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Notes
Although we did not have any specific a priori hypotheses about the BAS subscale comparisons for the combined diagnostic group versus the CTLs, it might be helpful interpret study findings within the context of these comparisons. Upon closer examination, the lack of a significant difference between the diagnostic groups and the CTLs on the BAS subscales may have been driven by a pattern of scores from the BD subsample that were somewhat similar to those reported by the CTLs (see Table 2). In comparison to prior research demonstrating significantly greater BAS sensitivity in at-risk or remitted individuals with BD versus healthy controls (Johnson and Carver 2006; Salavert et al. 2007), these findings point to a potential dampening of BAS sensitivity for BD when in a depressive episode, to levels similar to those reported by CTLs, but not as low as those reported by individuals with MDD. Future research will be necessary to further explore this possibility.
References
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.
Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Walshaw, P. D., Cogswell, A., Grandin, L. D., Hughes, M. E., et al. (2008). Behavioral approach system and behavioral inhibition system sensitivities and bipolar spectrum disorders: Prospective prediction of bipolar mood episodes. Bipolar Disorders, 10, 310–322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00547.x.
Alloy, L. B., Bender, R. E., Whitehouse, W. G., Wagner, C. A., Liu, R. T., Grant, D. A., et al. (2012). High behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity, reward responsiveness, and goal-striving predict first onset of bipolar spectrum disorders: A prospective behavioral high-risk design. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 339–351. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025877.
Alloy, L. B., Nusslock, R., & Boland, E. M. (2015). The development and course of bipolar spectrum disorders: An integrated reward and circadian rhythm dysregulation model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 213–250. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112902.
Altman, E. G., Hedeker, D., Peterson, J. L., & Davis, J. M. (1997). The Altman self-rating mania scale. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 948–955. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00548-3.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349.
Babor, T. F., Higgins-Biddle, J. C., Saunders, J. B., & Monteiro, M. G. (2001). The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for use in primary care. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300.
Berman, A. H., Bergman, H., Palmstierna, T., & Schlyter, F. (2003). The drug use disorders identification test manual. Stockholm: Karolinska Institute.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., et al. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therarpy, 42, 676–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007.
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319.
Chase, H. W., Nusslock, R., Almeida, J. R., Forbes, E. E., Labarbara, E. J., & Phillips, M. L. (2013). Dissociable patterns of abnormal frontal cortical activation during anticipation of an uncertain reward or loss in bipolar versus major depression. Bipolar Disorders, 15, 839–854. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12132.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Cribb, G., Moulds, M. L., & Carter, S. (2006). Rumination and experiential avoidance in depression. Behaviour Change, 23, 165–176. https://doi.org/10.1375/bech.23.3.165.
Cuellar, A. K., Johnson, S. L., & Winters, R. (2005). Distinctions between bipolar and unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 307–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.12.002.
De Almeida, J. R. C., & Phillips, M. L. (2013). Distinguishing between unipolar depression and bipolar depression: Current and future clinical and neuroimaging perspectives. Biological Psychiatry, 73, 111–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.010.
Depue, R. A., & Iacono, W. G. (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 457–492. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.002325.
Dickson, K. S., Ciesla, J. a., & Reilly, L. C. (2012). Rumination, worry, cognitive avoidance, and behavioral avoidance: Examination of temporal effects. Behavior Therapy, 43, 629–640. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.11.002.
Feldman, G. C., Joormann, J., & Johnson, S. L. (2008). Responses to positive affect: A self-report measure of rumination and dampening. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 507–525. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9083-0.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.
Ghaemi, N., Sachs, G. S., & Goodwin, F. K. (2000). What is to be done? Controversies in the diagnosis and treatment of manic-depressive illness. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 1, 65–74. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622970009150569.
Gilbert, K. E., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Gruber, J. (2013). Positive emotion dysregulation across mood disorders: How amplifying versus dampening predicts emotional reactivity and illness course. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 736–741. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.08.004.
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of pesronality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A Model for Personality (pp. 246–276). Berlin: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-67783-0_8.
Gruber, J., Eidelman, P., Johnson, S. L., Smith, B., & Harvey, A. G. (2011). Hooked on a feeling: Rumination about positive and negative emotion in inter-episode bipolar disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 956–961. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023667.
Hasler, G., Drevets, W. C., Gould, T. D., Gottesman, I. I., & Manji, H. K. (2006). Toward constructing an endophenotype strategy for bipolar disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 93–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.006.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., et al. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553–578.
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1152–1168. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1152.
Joffe, R. T., Young, L. T., & MacQueen, G. M. (1999). A two-illness model of bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 1, 25–30. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1399-5618.1999.10107.x.
Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (2006). Extreme goal setting and vulnerability to mania among undiagnosed young adults. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 377–395. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9044-7.
Johnson, S. L., McKenzie, G., & McMurrich, S. (2008). Ruminative responses to negative and positive affect among students diagnosed with bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 702–713. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9158-6.
Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H., & Ryan, C. S. (2009). Data analysis: A model comparison approach (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203892053.
Judd, L. L., Akiskal, H. S., Schettler, P. J., Endicott, J., Leon, A. C., Solomon, D. A., et al. (2005). Psychosocial disability in the course of bipolar I and II disorders: A prospective, comparative, longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 1322–1330. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1322.
Judd, L. L., Akiskal, H. S., Schettler, P. J., Endicott, J., Maser, J., Solomon, D. A., et al. (2002). The long-term natural history of the weekly symptomatic status of bipolar I disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 530–537. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.6.530.
Kasch, K. L., Rottenberg, J., Arnow, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002). Behavioral activation and inhibition systems and the severity and course of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 589–597. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.589.
Kashdan, T. B., Breen, W. E., Afram, A., & Terhar, D. (2010). Experiential avoidance in idiographic, autobiographical memories: Construct validity and links to social anxiety, depressive, and anger symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 528–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.03.010.
Lewinsohn, P. M. (1974). A behavioral approach to depression. In R. J. Friendman & M. M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 157–178). Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.
Manos, R. C., Kanter, J. W., & Luo, W. (2011). The Behavioral Activation for Depression Scale Short Form: Development and validation. Behavior Therapy, 42, 726–739. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.04.004.
Meyer, B., Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (1999). Exploring behavioral activation and inhibition sensitivities among college students at risk for bipolar spectrum symptomatology. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 21, 275–292. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022119414440.
Meyer, B., Johnson, S. L., & Winters, R. (2001). Responsiveness to threat and incentive in bipolar disorder: Relations of the BIS/BAS scales with symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 133–143. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010929402770.
Millgram, Y., Joorman, J., Huppert, J. D., & Tamir, M. (2015). Sad as a matter of choice? Emotion-regulation goals in depression. Psychological Science, 26, 1216–1228. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615583295.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569.
Pinto-Meza, A., Caseras, X., Soler, J., Puigdemont, D., Perez, V., & Torrubia, R. (2006). Behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation systems in current and recovered major depression participants. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 215–226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.021.
Redlich, R., Dohm, K., Grotegerd, D., Opel, N., Zwitserlood, P., Heindel, W., et al. (2015). Reward processing in unipolar and bipolar depression: A functional MRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 2623–2631. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.110.
Rochefort, C., Baldwin, A. S., & Chmielewski, M. (2017). Experiential avoidance: An examination of the construct validity of the AAQ-II and MEAQ. Behavior Therapy, 49, 435–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2017.08.008.
Ross, S. R., Millis, S. R., Bonebright, T. L., & Bailley, S. E. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of the behavioral inhibition and activation scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 861–865. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00196-9.
Rottenberg, J. (2017). Emotions in depression: What do we really know? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13, 241–263. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045252.
Salavert, J., Caseras, X., Torrubia, R., Furest, S., Arranz, B., Duenas, R., & San, L. (2007). The functioning of the behavioral activation and inhibition systems in bipolar I euthymic patients and its influence in subsequent episodes over an eighteen-month period. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1323–1331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.010.
Schaffer, A., Isometsä, E. T., Tondo, L., Moreno, H., Turecki, D., Reis, G., et al. (2015). International Society for Bipolar Disorders Task Force on Suicide: Meta-analyses and meta-regression of correlates of suicide attempts and suicide deaths in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 17, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12271.
Shahar, B., & Herr, N. R. (2011). Depressive symptoms predict inflexibly high levels of experiential avoidance in response to daily negative affect: A daily diary study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 676–681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.07.006.
Shapero, B. G., Stange, J. P., Goldstein, K. E., Black, C. L., Molz, A. R., Hamlat, E. J., et al. (2015). Cognitive styles in mood disorders: Discriminative ability of unipolar and bipolar cognitive profiles. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 8, 35–60. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2015.8.1.35.
Smith, D. J., & Craddock, N. (2011). Unipolar and bipolar depression: Different of the same? British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 272–274. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.111.092726.
Trew, J. L. (2011). Exploring the roles of approach and avoidance in depression: An integrative model. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 1156–1168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.007.
Trivedi, M. H., Rush, A. J., Ibrahim, H. M., Carmody, T. J., Biggs, M. M., Suppes, T., et al. (2004). The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Clinician Rating (IDS-C) and Self-Report (IDS-SR), and the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Clinician Rating (QIDS-C) and Self-Report (QIDS-SR) in public sector patients with mood disorders: A psychometric evaluation. Psychological Medicine, 34, 73–82. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291703001107.
Weinstock, L. M., Strong, D., Uebelacker, L. A., & Miller, I. W. (2009). Differential item functioning of DSM-IV depressive symptoms in individuals with a history of mania versus those without: An item response theory analysis. Bipolar Disorders, 11, 289–297. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00681.x.
Weinstock, L. M., Strong, D., Uebelacker, L. A., & Miller, I. W. (2010a). DSM-IV depressive symptom expression among individuals with a history of hypomania: A comparison to those with or without a history of mania. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44, 979–985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.010.
Weinstock, L. M., Strong, D., Uebelacker, L. A., & Miller, I. W. (2010b). Use of item response theory methodology to evaluate differences between bipolar and unipolar depression. US Psychiatry, 3, 15–20.
Whiteford, H. A., Degenhardt, L., Rehm, J., Baxter, A. J., Ferrari, A. J., Erskine, H. E., … Vos, T. (2013). Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: Findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. The Lancet, 382, 1575–1586. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61611-6.
Funding
This research was supported in part by Grant K23MH079907 from the National Institute of Mental Health (LW) and Grant UL1RR024139 from the National Center for Research Resources and the National Center for Advancing Translational Science (JG).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
Lauren M. Weinstock, Tina Chou, Cintly Celis-deHoyos, Ivan W. Miller, and June Gruber declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Informed Consent
All procedures were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional review committees of Butler Hospital and Brown University, Providence, RI, USA, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Animal Rights
No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Weinstock, L.M., Chou, T., Celis-deHoyos, C. et al. Reward and Punishment Sensitivity and Emotion Regulation Processes Differentiate Bipolar and Unipolar Depression. Cogn Ther Res 42, 794–802 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-9945-2
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-9945-2
Keywords
- Bipolar disorder
- Major depressive disorder
- Reward sensitivity
- Punishment sensitivity
- Emotion regulation