Advertisement

Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 42, Issue 1, pp 48–62 | Cite as

Distress Intolerance Moderation of Attention to Emotion: An Eye-Tracking Study

  • Richard J. Macatee
  • Katherine A. McDermott
  • Brian J. Albanese
  • Norman B. Schmidt
  • Jesse R. CougleEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

Distress intolerance (DI) is an important individual difference reflective of the inability to endure aversive affective states and is relevant to multiple clinical populations, but underlying emotional processing mechanisms remain unclear. The current study used eye-tracking to examine biased attention towards emotional stimuli at baseline and in the context of acute stress in a non-clinical sample (N = 165). We hypothesized that DI would incrementally predict greater stressor-elicited increases in sustained/delayed disengagement, but not initial orientation/facilitated engagement negative (i.e., threat, dysphoric) attention biases, and that DI’s association with maladaptive stress regulation would depend on these increases. Partially consistent with predictions, DI was only independently associated with stressor-elicited increases in sustained negative bias and, unexpectedly, decreases in sustained positive bias. Further, DI and change in sustained threat bias marginally interacted to predict cardiovascular but not subjective anxious mood recovery. Theoretical implications are discussed.

Keywords

Attentional bias Eye-tracking Distress intolerance Mood induction Emotion 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Richard J. Macatee, Katherine A. McDermott, Brian J. Albanese, Norman B. Schmidt, and Jesse R. Cougle declares that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Animal Rights

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Supplementary material

10608_2017_9869_MOESM1_ESM.docx (28 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 28 KB)

References

  1. Abrantes, A. M., Strong, D. R., Lejuez, C. W., Kahler, C. W., Carpenter, L. L., Price, L. H., ... & Brown, R. A. (2008). The role of negative affect in risk for early lapse among low distress tolerance smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 33(11), 1394–1401.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Allan, N. P., Macatee, R. J., Norr, A. M., & Schmidt, N. B. (2014). Direct and interactive effects of distress tolerance and anxiety sensitivity on generalized anxiety and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(5), 30–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, E. R., & Hope, D. A. (2009). The relationship among social phobia, objective and perceived physiological reactivity, and anxiety sensitivity in an adolescent population. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(1), 18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.03.011.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Armstrong, T., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(8), 704–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Bornovalova, M. A., Gratz, K. L., Daughters, S. B., Hunt, E. D., & Lejuez, C. W. (2012). Initial RCT of a distress tolerance treatment for individuals with substance use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 122(1), 70–76.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown, R. A., Lejuez, C. W., Strong, D. R., Kahler, C. W., Zvolensky, M. J., Carpenter, L. L., Niaura, R., & Price, L. H. (2009). A prospective examination of distress tolerance and early smoking lapse in adult self-quitters. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 11(5), 493–502. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Burt, K. B., & Obradovic, J. (2013). The construct of psychophysiological reactivity: Statistical and psychometric issues. Developmental Review, 33(1), 29–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 203–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Clasen, P. C., Wells, T. T., Ellis, A. J., & Beevers, C. G. (2013). Attentional biases and the persistence of sad mood in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(1), 74. doi: 10.1037/a0029211.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Cougle, J. R., Timpano, K. R., Fitch, K. E., & Hawkins, K. A. (2011). Distress tolerance and obsessions: an integrative analysis. Depression & Anxiety, 28(10), 906–914. doi: 10.1002/da.20846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cummings, J. R., Bornovalova, M. A., Ojanen, T., Hunt, E., MacPherson, L., & Lejuez, C. (2013). Time doesn’t change everything: The longitudinal course of distress tolerance and its relationship with externalizing and internalizing symptoms during early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(5), 735–748.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Cyders, M. A., & Coskunpinar, A. (2011). Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: Is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clinical psychology review, 31(6), 965–982.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Daughters, S. B., Lejuez, C. W., Bornovalova, M. A., Kahler, C. W., Strong, D. R., & Brown, R. A. (2005). Distress tolerance as a predictor of early treatment dropout in a residential substance abuse treatment facility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 729–734. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.729.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Eder, H., Fischer, G., Gombas, W., Jagsch, R., Stuhlinger, G., & Kasper, S. (1998). Comparison of buprenorphine and methadone maintenance in opiate addicts. European Addiction Research, 4(Suppl. 1), 3–7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Gibb, B. E., McGeary, J. E., & Beevers, C. G. (2016). Attentional biases to emotional stimuli: Key components of the RDoC constructs of sustained threat and loss. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(1), 65–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gorka, S. M., Lieberman, L., Phan, K. L., & Shankman, S. A. (2016). Association between problematic alcohol use and reactivity to uncertain threat in two independent samples. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 164(1), 89–96.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Gouin, J., Deschênes, S. S., & Dugas, M. J. (2014). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia during worry forecasts stress-related increases in psychological distress. Stress: The International Journal On The Biology Of Stress, 17(5), 416–422. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2014.949666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Graham, R., Hoover, A., Ceballos, N. A., & Komogortsev, O. (2011). Body mass index moderates gaze orienting biases and pupil diameter to high and low calorie food images. Appetite, 56(3), 577–586.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Gurka, M. J., Edwards, L. J., & Muller, K. E. (2011). Avoiding bias in mixed model inference for fixed effects. Statistics in Medicine, 30(22), 2696–2707.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Hogle, J. M., Kaye, J. T., & Curtin, J. J. (2010). Nicotine withdrawal increases threat-induced anxiety but not fear: neuroadaptation in human addiction. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 719–725.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review, 99(3), 561–565.  10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.561.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Johnson, D. R. (2009a). Emotional attention set-shifting and its relationship to anxiety and emotion regulation. Emotion (Washington, D. C.), 9(5), 681–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Johnson, D. R. (2009b). Goal-directed attentional deployment to emotional faces and individual differences in emotional regulation. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(1), 8–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kellough, J., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A., & Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: An eye tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1238–1243. doi:  10.1016/j.brat.2008.07.004.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Keogh, E., & Reidy, J. (2000). Exploring the factor structure of the mood and anxiety symptom questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 74(1), 106–125.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Kiselica, A. M., Rojas, E., Bornovalova, M. A., & Dube, C. (2015). The nomological network of self- reported distress tolerance. Assessment, 22(6), 715–729.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Kiselica, A. M., Webber, T., & Bornovalova, M. A. (2014). Stability and change in distress tolerance and its prospective relationship with borderline personality features: A short-term longitudinal study. Personality Disorders, 5(3), 247–256. doi:  10.1037/per0000076.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Leyro, T. M., Zvolensky, M. J., & Bernstein, A. (2010). Distress tolerance and psychopathological symptoms and disorders: a review of the empirical literature among adults. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 576–600.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Lintzeris, N., Bell, J., Bammer, G., Jolley, D. J., & Rushworth, L. (2002). A randomized controlled trial of buprenorphine in the management of short-term ambulatory heroin withdrawal. Addiction 97(11), 1395–1404.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces - KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet., ISBN 91-630-7164-9.Google Scholar
  31. Macatee, R. J., Albanese, B. J., Allan, N. P., Schmidt, N. B., & Cougle, J. R. (2016). Distress intolerance as a moderator of the relationship between daily stressors and affective symptoms: Tests of incremental and prospective relationships. Journal of Affective Disorders, 206, 125–132.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Macatee, R. J., Albanese, B. J., Schmidt, N. B., & Cougle, J. R. (2017a). Attention bias towards negative emotional information and its relationship with daily worry in the context of acute stress: An eye-tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 90, 96–110.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Macatee, R. J., Albanese, B. J., Schmidt, N. B., & Cougle, J. R. (2017b). The moderating influence of heart rate variability on stressor-elicited change in pupillary and attentional indices of emotional processing: An eye-tracking study. Biological Psychology, 123, 83–93.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Macatee, R. J., Capron, D. W., Guthrie, W., Schmidt, N. B., & Cougle, J. R. (2015). Distress tolerance and pathological worry: Tests of incremental and prospective relationships. Behavior Therapy, 46(4), 449–462. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2015.03.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Macatee, R. J., Capron, D. W., Schmidt, N. B., & Cougle, J. R. (2013). An examination of low distress tolerance and life stressors as factors underlying obsessions. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(10), 1462–1468. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.06.019.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Macatee, R. J., & Cougle, J. R. (2015). Development and evaluation of a computerized intervention for low distress tolerance and its effect on performance on a neutralization task. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 48, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.01.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Malooly, A. M., Genet, J. J., & Siemer, M. (2013). Individual differences in reappraisal effectiveness: The role of affective flexibility. Emotion, 13(2), 302.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 455–470.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. McHugh, R. K., & Otto, M. W. (2011). Domain-general and domain-specific strategies for the assessment of distress intolerance. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25, 745–749. doi: 10.1037/a0025094.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. McHugh, R. K., & Otto, M. W. (2012). Refining the measurement of distress intolerance. Behavior Therapy, 43, 641–651.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. McHugh, R. K., Weiss, R. D., Cornelius, M., Martel, M. O., Jamison, R. N., & Edwards, R. R. (2016). Distress intolerance and prescription opioid misuse among patients with chronic pain. The Journal of Pain, 17(7), 806–814.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Nelson, A. L., Purdon, C., Quigley, L., Carriere, J., & Smilek, D. (2015). Distinguishing the roles of trait and state anxiety on the nature of anxiety-related attentional biases to threat using a free viewing eye movement paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 29(3), 504–526.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Norberg, M. M., Keyan, D., & Grisham, J. R. (2015). Mood influences the relationship between distress intolerance and discarding. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 6, 77–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Patrick, C. J., Venables, N. C., Yancey, J. R., Hicks, B. M., Nelson, L. D., & Kramer, M. D. (2013). A construct-network approach to bridging diagnostic and physiological domains: application to assessment of externalizing psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 902.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Perkins, K. A., Karelitz, J. L., Giedgowd, G. E., Conklin, C. A., & Sayette, M. A. (2010). Differences in negative mood-induced smoking reinforcement due to distress tolerance, anxiety sensitivity, and depression history. Psychopharmacology, 210(1), 25–34.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Premack, D. (1959). Toward empirical behavior laws: I. Positive reinforcement. Psychological Review, 66(4), 219.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Price, R. B., Kuckertz, J. M., Siegle, G. J., Ladouceur, C. D., Silk, J. S., Ryan, N. D., ... & Amir, N. (2015). Empirical recommendations for improving the stability of the dot-probe task in clinical research. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 365.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Quené, H., & Van den Bergh, H. (2004). On multi-level modeling of data from repeated measures designs: A tutorial. Speech Communication, 43(1), 103–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Ramshur, J. T. (2010). Design, evaluation, and application of heart rate variability software (HRVAS) (Doctoral dissertation, Master’s thesis. The University of Memphis, Memphis, TN: Master’s thesis).Google Scholar
  50. Safren, S. A., Turk, C. L., & Heimberg, R. G. (1998). Factor structure of the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 36(4), 443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sanchez, A., & Vazquez, C. (2013). Prototypicality and intensity of emotional faces using an anchor- point method. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sanchez, A., Vazquez, C., Marker, C., LeMoult, J., & Joormann, J. (2013). Attentional disengagement predicts stress recovery in depression: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(2), 303–313. doi: 10.1037/a0031529.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Seo, J. W., & Kwon, S. M. (2016). Testing an affective judgment model of distress tolerance in college heavy drinkers. Addictive behaviors, 58, 100–103.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Shafqat, K., Pal, S. K., & Kyriacou, P. A. (2007). Evaluation of two detrending techniques for application in heart rate variability. Proceedings of the 29th Annual International Conference of the IEEE EMBS, Cite Internationale Lyon, France, ISBN no 1-4244-0788-5.Google Scholar
  55. Shaw, A. M., & Timpano, K. R. (2016). An experimental investigation of the effect of stress on saving and acquiring behavioral tendencies: The role of distress tolerance and negative urgency. Behavior therapy, 47(1), 116–129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. SR Research Ltd. (2005–2010). EyeLink 1000 User Manual (version 1.5.2).Google Scholar
  57. Strong, D. R., Brown, R. A., Sims, M., Herman, D. S., Anderson, B. J., & Stein, M. D. (2012). Persistence during stress-challenge associated with lapse to opioid use during buprenorphine treatment. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 6(3), 219–225.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. Strong, D. R., Lejuez, C. W., Daughters, S., Marinello, M., Kahler, C. W., & Brown, R. A. (2003). The computerized mirror tracing task, version 1.Unpublished manual.Google Scholar
  59. Szuhany, K. L., & Otto, M. W. (2015). Contextual influences on distress intolerance: Priming effects on behavioral persistence. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(4), 499–507.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  60. Trafton, J. A., & Gifford, E. V. (2011). Biological Bases of Distress Tolerance. In M. J. Zvolensky, A. Bernstein & A. A. Vujanovic (Eds.), Distress Tolerance: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications (pp. 80–104). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
  61. Volz, A. R., Dennis, P. A., Dennis, M. F., Calhoun, P. S., Wilson, S. M., & Beckham, J. C. (2014). The role of daily hassles and distress tolerance in predicting cigarette craving during a quit attempt. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 16(6), 872–875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Waechter, S., Nelson, A. L., Wright, C., Hyatt, A., & Oakman, J. (2014). Measuring attentional bias to threat: Reliability of dot probe and eye movement indices. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(3), 313–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Waugh, C. E., Panage, S., Mendes, W. B., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Cardiovascular and affective recovery from anticipatory threat. Biological Psychology, 84, 169–175. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.010.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  64. Williams, C. L., Vik, P. W., & Wong, M. M. (2015). Distress tolerance in social versus solitary college student drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 50, 89–95.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyFlorida State UniversityTallahasseeUSA

Personalised recommendations