Skip to main content
Log in

Are Depressed People More or Less Susceptible to Informational Social Influence?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When making judgments and decisions, people suffering from depression are often faced with opinions and advice from others (e.g., from their therapists) but it is unclear how their psychopathology alters the utilization of such information. This study is the first to examine whether depressed people are more or less susceptible to informational social influence. To this end, we employed the Judge–Advisor-System, which allows for a pure test of how people utilize information from others. We found that depressed participants had significantly higher advice taking values than non-depressed participants, which was mediated by self-esteem. A fine-grained analysis of these group differences revealed that depressed participants were more likely to revise their initial estimates after receiving advice than non-depressed people. Yet, once having decided to revise their estimates, depressed people did not weight advice more heavily. Theoretical implications concerning two qualitatively independent effects of depression on advice taking are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Admittedly, our sample size was rather small due to a limited number of eligible patients. We will further discuss this in the “Limitations” section.

  2. Other disorders like an anxiety disorder were permitted in the control group. Actually, just one participant in this group fulfilled the criteria for a lifetime diagnosis (panic disorder) but was remitted during study participation.

  3. We did not compensate the inpatient group with financial incentives because they were tested in the local hospital where a number of other studies were conducted. Since it is not common to compensate participants in this institution, we did not want to make any exceptions to this rule. Previous studies found that financial incentives reduce advice discounting (Bonaccio and Dalal 2006), so this inequality in payment could lead to a higher advice utilization in the control group. Therefore, the possible confound produces an effect in the opposite direction of the one postulated in our hypothesis.

  4. A Levene’s test revealed that the variance of both groups was not equal. Therefore, we report the results with corrected degrees of freedom.

  5. Another possibility to deal with such AT values is to change negative values to 0 and values above 1 to 1 (e.g., Gino 2008; Gino et al. 2009; Schultze et al. 2015). Hence, we re-ran the analyses, truncating the AT scores at 0 and 1 instead of excluding extreme values. These analyses yielded the same pattern of results.

  6. Self-esteem values were missing for five participants. Therefore, we removed these participants from model 2 in order to allow comparison of this model with models 3 and 4. Removing these participants did not change the pattern of results reported for models 1 and 2 in Table 2.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1955). Opinions and social pressure. Readings About the Social Animal, 193, 17–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70(9), 1–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck depression inventory. Manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beevers, C. G., Clasen, P. C., Enock, P. M., & Schnyer, D. M. (2015). Attention bias modification for major depressive disorder: Effects on attention bias, resting state connectivity, and symptom change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 463–475. doi:10.1037/abn0000049.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bonaccio, S., & Dalal, R. S. (2006). Advice taking and decision-making: An integrative literature review, and implications for the organizational sciences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 101(2), 127–151. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourke, C., Douglas, K., & Porter, R. (2010). Processing of facial emotion expression in major depression: A review. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44(8), 681–696. doi:10.3109/00048674.2010.496359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Busch, A. M., Kanter, J. W., Landes, S. J., & Kohlenberg, R. J. (2006). Sudden gains and outcome: A broader temporal analysis of cognitive therapy for depression. Behavior Therapy, 37(1), 61–68. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2005.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry, 39, 28–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cruwys, T., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G. A., Haslam, C., & Jetten, J. (2014). Depression and social identity: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(3), 215–238. doi:10.1177/1088868314523839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influence upon individual judgment. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 629–636.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, P. (2000). The relationship of shame, social anxiety and depression: The role of the evaluation of social rank. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 7(3), 174–189. doi:10.1002/1099-0879(200007)7:3<174::AID-CPP236>3.0.CO;2-U.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F. (2008). Do we listen to advice just because we paid for it? The impact of advice cost on its use. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 107(2), 234–245. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Brooks, A. W., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2012). Anxiety, advice, and the ability to discern: Feeling anxious motivates individuals to seek and use advice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 497–512. doi:10.1037/a0026413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2008). Blinded by anger or feeling the love: How emotions influence advice taking. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(5), 1165–1173. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.5.1165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Shang, J., & Croson, R. (2009). The impact of information from similar or different advisors on judgment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(2), 287–302. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, K. M., & Crisp, D. A. (2013). Unmet depression information needs in the community. Journal of Affective Disorders, 146(3), 348–354. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.09.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hames, J. L., Hagan, C. R., & Joiner, T. E. (2013). Interpersonal processes in depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 355–377. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, N., & Fischer, I. (1997). Taking advice: Accepting help, improving judgment, and sharing responsibility. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(2), 117–133. doi:10.1006/obhd.1997.2697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hautzinger, M., Keller, F., & Kuehner, C. (2006). Beck Depression Inventar (BDI-II). Deutsche Bearbeitung und Handbuch. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofheinz, C. (2009). Problematische Internetnutzung und ihre negativen Folgen: Gibt es einen Zusammenhang zu sozialer Ängstlichkeit und dem Ausmaß an privater Internetnutzung? (Unpublished bachelor’s thesis). Göttingen: University of Göttingen.

  • Janis, I. L. (1954). Personality correlates of susceptibility to persuasion. Journal of Personality, 22(4), 504–518. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1954.tb01870.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E. (2000). Depression’s vicious scree: Self-propagating and erosive processes in depression chronicity. Clinical Psychology Science and Practice, 7(2), 203–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., Alfano, M. S., & Metalsky, G. I. (1993). Caught in the crossfire: Depression, self-consistency, self-enhancement, and the response of others. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12(2), 113–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., Metalsky, G. I., Katz, J., & Beach, S. R. H. (2009). Depression and excessive reassurance-seeking. Psychological Inquiry, 10(3), 269–278. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1004_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaliuzhna, M., Chambon, V., Franck, N., Testud, B., & Van der Henst, J.-B. (2012). Belief revision and delusions: How do patients with schizophrenia take advice? PLoS One, 7(4), e34771. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034771.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9(3), 371–375. doi:10.1177/0146167283093007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leykin, Y., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2010). Decision-making styles and depressive symptomatology: Development of the decision styles questionnaire. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(7), 506–515. doi:10.1002/9780470618837.ch.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leykin, Y., Roberts, C. S., & Derubeis, R. J. (2011). Decision-making and depressive symptomatology. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35(4), 333–341. doi:10.1007/s10608-010-9308-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manicavasagar, V., Perich, T., & Parker, G. (2012). Cognitive predictors of change in cognitive behaviour therapy and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 40(2), 227–232. doi:10.1017/S1352465811000634.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mansell, W., & Lam, D. (2006). “I Won’t Do What You Tell Me!”: Elevated mood and the assessment of advice-taking in euthymic bipolar I disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1787–1801. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.01.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCullough, J. P. (2000). Treatment for chronic depression: Cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy (CBASP). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moise, N., Ye, S., Alcantara, C., Davidson, K. W., & Kronish, I. (2017). Depressive symptoms and decision-making preferences in patients with comorbid illnesses. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 92, 63–66. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.12.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moyse, E. (2014). Age estimation from faces and voices: A review. Psychologica Belgica, 54(3), 255–265. doi:10.5334/pb.aq.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, C. J. L., Vos, T., Lozano, R., Naghavi, M., Flaxman, A. D., Michaud, C., et al. (2012). Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 291 diseases and injuries in 21 regions, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. The Lancet, 380(9859), 2197–2223. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61689-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Okwumabua, J. O., Wong, S. P., & Duryea, E. J. (2003). Depressive symptoms and decision making among African American youth. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18(5), 436–453. doi:10.1177/0743558403255062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, 36(4), 717–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, M. H., Nakane, Y., Ohta, Y., Mann, L., & Kalucy, R. S. (1991). Decision making in clinically depressed patients. A transcultural social psychology study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179(12), 711–719.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, N., & Wood, W. (1992). Self-esteem and intelligence affect influenceability: The mediating role of message reception. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 156–171. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, D. (2011). Prevalence and clinical course of depression: a review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(7), 1117–1125. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., Schoenbach, C., & Rosenberg, F. (1995). Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, different outcomes. American Sociological Review, 60(1), 141–156. doi:10.2307/2096350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanderson, W. C., Beck, A. T., & Beck, J. (1990). Syndrome comorbidity in patients with major depression or dysthymia: Prevalence and temporal relationships. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(8), 1025–1028. doi:10.1176/ajp.147.8.1025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609–612. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultze, T., Rakotoarisoa, A.-F., & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2015). Effects of distance between initial estimates and advice on advice utilization. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(2), 144–171. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2015-14259-003&site=ehost-live.

  • Sniezek, J. A., & Buckley, T. (1995). Cueing and cognitive conflict in judge-advisor decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62(2), 159–174. doi:10.1006/obhd.1995.1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soll, J. B., & Larrick, R. P. (2009). Strategies for revising judgment: How (and how well) people use others’ opinions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 780–805. doi:10.1037/a0015145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soll, J. B., & Mannes, A. E. (2011). Judgmental aggregation strategies depend on whether the self is involved. International Journal of Forecasting, 27(1), 81–102. doi:10.1016/j.ijforecast.2010.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sowislo, J. F., & Orth, U. (2013). Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 213–240. doi:10.1037/a0028931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W. B., Wenzlaff, R. M., Krull, D. S., & Pelham, B. W. (1992). Allure of negative feedback: Self-verification strivings among depressed persons. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(2), 293–306. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.101.2.293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, A. J. W., & Vaughan, G. M. (1967). Clinical depression and conformity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 25(1), 257–260. doi:10.2466/pms.1967.25.1.257.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tlach, L., Wusten, C., Daubmann, A., Liebherz, S., Harter, M., & Dirmaier, J. (2015). Information and decision-making needs among people with mental disorders: A systematic review of the literature. Health Expectations: An International Journal of Public Participation in Health Care and Health Policy, 18(6), 1856–1872. doi:10.1111/hex.12251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tost, L. P., Gino, F., & Larrick, R. P. (2012). Power, competitiveness, and advice taking: Why the powerful don’t listen. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117(1), 53–65. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.10.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tse, W. S., & Bond, A. J. (2004). The impact of depression on social skills: A review. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192(4), 260–268. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000120884.60002.2b.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Collani, G., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 24(1), 3–7. doi:10.1024//0170-1789.24.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, J. E., Becker, J., Coppel, D. B., & Cox, G. B. (1983). Anticonformity aspects of depression in mild depressive states. Journal of Personality, 51(4), 640–652. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1983.tb00871.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weightman, M. J., Air, T. M., & Baune, B. T. (2014). A review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 179. doi:10.1037/t04100-000.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wittchen, H.-U., Wunderlich, U., Gruschwitz, S., & Zaudig, M. (1997). SKID-I. Strukturiertes klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen. Interviewheft [SCID-I. Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV. Axis I disorders. Users guide]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (2012). Depression. A Global Public Health Concern. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/who_paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf.

  • Yaniv, I. (2004). The benefit of additional opinions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 75–78. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00278.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yaniv, I., & Kleinberger, E. (2000). Advice taking in decision making: egocentric discounting and reputation formation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83(2), 260–281. doi:10.1006/obhd.2000.2909.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zettle, R. D., Rains, J. C., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). Processes of change in acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive therapy for depression: A mediation reanalysis of Zettle and Rains. Behavior Modification, 35(3), 265–283. doi:10.1177/0145445511398344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The research reported in this article was supported by a grant from the Volkswagen Foundation for the project “Social conformity: Why do humans and monkeys make weak decisions under social influence?” (Az. 85 148) within the European Platform for Life Sciences, Mind Sciences, and the Humanities.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christine Hofheinz.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Christine Hofheinz, Markus Germar, Thomas Schultze, Johannes Michalak and Andreas Mojzisch declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in the study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the local ethics committee.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (national and institutional). Informed consent was obtained from all individual subjects participating in the study.

Animal Rights Statement

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hofheinz, C., Germar, M., Schultze, T. et al. Are Depressed People More or Less Susceptible to Informational Social Influence?. Cogn Ther Res 41, 699–711 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9848-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9848-7

Keywords

Navigation