Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitive Control in Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Relation of Inhibition Impairments to Worry and Anxiety Severity

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive models of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) propose that cognitive control, broadly construed, and inhibition specifically, play a role in the maintenance of GAD symptoms. However, few studies have explicitly investigated inhibition, and in particular “cold” (non-emotional) inhibition, and its relation to worry and anxiety severity in GAD. Adults with GAD (n = 35) and healthy controls (n = 21) completed computerized Stroop and Go/NoGo tasks, two widely-used tests of inhibition. GAD status predicted significantly worse (slower and less accurate) performance on the Stroop but not the Go/NoGo task. Clinician-rated anxiety severity predicted slower and less accurate Stroop performance over and above the effect of GAD diagnosis but did not predict Go/NoGo performance. Trait worry did not incrementally predict performance on either task. These findings provide qualified support for theoretical models of inhibition impairments in GAD and suggest that inhibition could be a promising target for novel neurocognitive interventions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

References

  • Abramovitch, A., Abramowitz, J. S., & Mittelman, A. (2013). The neuropsychology of adult obsessive–compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(8), 1163–1171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abramovitch, A., Shaham, N., Levin, L., Bar-Hen, M., & Schweiger, A. (2015). Response inhibition in a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 46, 66–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: Automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(1), 49–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk & D. S. Mennin (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 77–108). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkovec, T. D., & Inz, J. (1990). The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: A predominance of thought activity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(2), 153–158.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. A., Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1992). Psychometric properties of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire in a clinical anxiety disorders sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30(1), 33–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. A., DiNardo, P. A., Lehman, C. L., & Campbell, L. A. (2001). Reliability of DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: Implications for the classification of emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 49–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2010). The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 264–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doniger, G. (2011). Mindstreams: Survey Responses. In support of P. J. Snyder, C. E. Jackson, R. C. Petersen, A. S. Khachaturian, J. Kaye, M. Albert, S. Weintraub (2011). The PAD2020 Survey of Cognitive Instruments for the Study of Mild Cognitive Impairment. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association. Retrieved from http://www.pad2020.org/CTCR/CTCRSummaries/.

  • Doniger, G. M., Simon, E. S., & Schweiger, A. (2008). Adjustment of cognitive scores with a co-normed estimate of premorbid intelligence: Implementation using Mindstreams computerized testing. Applied Neuropsychology, 15, 250–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dwolatzky, T., Whitehead, V., Doniger, G. M., Simon, E. S., Schweiger, A., Jaffe, D., & Chertkow, H. (2003). Validity of a novel computerized cognitive battery for mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatrics, 3, 4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Enright, S. J., & Beech, A. R. (1993). Reduced cognitive inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32(1), 67–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(7), 955–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion (Washington, D. C.), 7(2), 336–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guy, W. (1976). Assessment manual for psychopharmacology: US Government Printing Office.

  • Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6), 940–958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2013). Should uncontrollable worry be removed from the definition of GAD? A test of incremental validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(2), 369–375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallion, L.S., Ruscio, A. M., & Jha, A. P. (2014). Fractionating the role of executive control in control over worry: A preliminary investigation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 54, 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S., Hirsch, C., & Mathews, A. (2008). Restriction of working memory capacity during worry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(3), 712–717.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hegedish, O., Doniger, G. M., & Schweiger, A. (2012). Detecting response bias on the MindStreams Battery. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 19(2), 262–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, C. R., & Mathews, A. (2012). A cognitive model of pathological worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(10), 636–646.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J. (2006). Differential effects of rumination and dysphoria on the inhibition of irrelevant emotional material: Evidence from a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 149–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J. (2010). Cognitive inhibition and emotion regulation in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 161–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 281–298.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Hertel, P. T., LeMoult, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Training forgetting of negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 34–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A. F. (2000). Does old age reduce the risk of anxiety and depression? A review of epidemiological studies across the adult life span. Psychological Medicine, 30(01), 11–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kindt, M., Bierman, D., & Brosschot, J. F. (1996). Stroop versus Stroop: Comparison of a card format and a single-trial format of the standard color-word Stroop task and the emotional Stroop task. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(5), 653–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, D. V., Weiller, E., Amorim, P., Bonora, I., Dunbar, G. C., et al. (1997). The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: Reliability and validity according to the CIDI. European Psychiatry, 12(5), 224–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, E., & Hirsch, C. R. (2011). Worry in imagery and verbal form: Effect on residual working memory capacity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(2), 99–105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Little, R. J. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • López-Pina, J. A., Sánchez-Meca, J., & Rosa-Alcázar, A. I. (2009). The Hamilton rating scale for depression: A meta-analytic reliability generalization study. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 9, 143–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 455–468.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNally, R. J. (1995). Automaticity and the anxiety disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(7), 747–754.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus major depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 29–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nee, D. E., Wager, T. D., & Jonides, J. (2007). Interference resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(1), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220–246.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., Howard, R. J., & Ballard, C. G. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465(7299), 775–778.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Peers, P. V., & Lawrence, A. D. (2009). Attentional control of emotional distraction in rapid serial visual presentation. Emotion (Washington, D. C.), 9(1), 140–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, R. B., & Mohlman, J. (2007). Inhibitory control and symptom severity in late life generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2628–2639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2003). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riccio, C. A., Reynolds, C. R., & Lowe, P. A. (2001). Clinical applications of continuous performance tests: Measuring attention and impulsive responding in children and adults. John Wiley & Sons Inc.

  • Roberts, R. J., Hager, L. D., & Heron, C. (1994). Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(4), 374–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweiger, A., Abramovitch, A., Doniger, G. M., & Simon, E. S. (2007). A clinical construct validity study of a novel computerized battery for the diagnosis of ADHD in young adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(1), 100–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shear, M. K., Vanderbilt, J., Rucci, P., Endicott, J., Lydiard, B., Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Chandler, L., Williams, J., Alli, A., & Frank, B. M. (2001). Reliability and validity of a structured guard for the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (SIGH-A). Anxiety, 13, 166–178.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Keskiner, A., et al. (1997). The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability. European Psychiatry, 12(5), 232–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 81–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stange, J. P., Connolly, S. L., Burke, T. A., Hamilton, J. L., Hamlat, E. J., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Inflexible cognition predicts first onset of major depressive episodes in adolescence. Depression and Anxiety,1–8.

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tempesta, D., Mazza, M., Serroni, N., Moschetta, F. S., Di Giannanto, M., Ferrara, M., & De Berardis, D. (2013). Neuropsychological functioning in young subjects with generalized anxiety disorder with and without pharmacotherapy. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 45, 236–241.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120(2), 272–292.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. B. (1988). A structured interview guide for the Hamilton depression rating scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(8), 742–747.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by Grant #129522 from the Hartford HealthCare Research Funding Initiative to G. J. D.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lauren S. Hallion.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Lauren S. Hallion declares no conflict of interest. David F. Tolin declares no conflict of interest. John Goethe declares no conflict of interest. Gretchen J. Diefenbach receives material support from Neuronetics for research.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hallion, L.S., Tolin, D.F., Assaf, M. et al. Cognitive Control in Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Relation of Inhibition Impairments to Worry and Anxiety Severity. Cogn Ther Res 41, 610–618 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9832-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9832-2

Keywords

Navigation