Skip to main content
Log in

Self-Punishment as a Maladaptive Thought Control Strategy Mediates the Relationship Between Beliefs About Thoughts and Repugnant Obsessions

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive-behavioral models of obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) propose that erroneous beliefs about the importance and need to control thoughts lead individuals with OCD to overuse maladaptive thought control strategies such as worry and self-punishment. These strategies are thought to backfire, leading to obsessional symptoms. Whereas previous studies have examined predictors of the use of thought control strategies, none have investigated these strategies as mediators in the prediction of OCD symptom severity. An additional gap in the literature is the lack of attention to OCD symptom dimensions. The present study, therefore, extended existing work by examining thought control strategies as mediators of the relationship between obsessive beliefs and OCD symptom dimensions in a treatment-seeking sample of 102 adults with OCD. In line with our hypotheses, the use of punishment (but not worry) as a thought control tactic mediated the relationship between dysfunctional beliefs about the importance/control of thoughts and unacceptable obsessions. These findings are discussed in terms of the conceptualization and treatment of OCD. Future research should investigate the extent to which these various thought control strategies may be helpful versus harmful using idiographic measures of thought control and employing experimental designs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Due to missing data, years of education was only available for a subset of the sample (n = 52).

References

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Deacon, B. J., Olatunji, B. O., Wheaton, M. G., Berman, N. C., Losardo, D., et al. (2010). Assessment of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions: Development and evaluation of the Dimensional Obsessive–Compulsive Scale. Psychological Assessment, 22(1), 180–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Fabricant, L. E., Taylor, S., Deacon, B. J., McKay, D., & Storch, E. A. (2014). The utility of analogue studies for understanding obsessions and compulsions. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(3), 206–217. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Khandker, M., Nelson, C. A., Deacon, B. J., & Rygwall, R. (2006). The role of cognitive factors in the pathogenesis of obsessive–compulsive symptoms: a prospective study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1361–1374. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.09.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Taylor, S., & McKay, D. (2009). Obsessive–compulsive disorder. The Lancet, 374(9688), 491–499. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60240-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F., & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(5), 683–703. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00057-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Whiteside, S., Kalsy, S. A., & Tolin, D. F. (2003). Thought control strategies in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A replication and extension. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(5), 529–540. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00026-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amir, N., Cashman, L., & Foa, E. B. (1997). Strategies of thought control in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 775–777. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00030-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belloch, A., Morillo, C., & Garcia-Soriano, G. (2009). Strategies to control unwanted intrusive thoughts: Which are relevant and specific in obsessive–compulsive disorder? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33(1), 75–89. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9141-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belloch, A., Morillo, C., Lucero, M., Cabedo, E., & Carrió, C. (2004). Intrusive thoughts in non-clinical subjects: The role of frequency and unpleasantness on appraisal ratings and control strategies. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 11(2), 100–110. doi:10.1002/cpp.397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A. (2004). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for OCD. New York; London: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., & de Silva, P. (1985). The nature of depressive and anxious, intrusive thoughts: Distinct or uniform phenomena? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(4), 383–393. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(85)90166-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., & Mazure, C. (1989a). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: I. Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 1006–1011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., & Mazure, C. (1989b). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: II. Validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 1012–1016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 1–25. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitz, L. J., & Purdon, C. (2008). Predictors and consequences of suppressing obsessional thoughts. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36(2), 179–192. doi:10.1017/S1352465807003992.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKay, D., Abramowitz, J. S., Calamari, J. E., Kyrios, M., Radomsky, A., Sookman, D., et al. (2004). A critical evaluation of obsessive–compulsive disorder subtypes: Symptoms versus mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review, 24(3), 283–313. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2004.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, E. L., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2007). The cognitive mediation of thought-control strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(8), 1949–1955. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (1997). Cognitive assessment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(7), 667–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (2001). Development and initial validation of the Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire and the Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39(8), 987–1006. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00085-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (2005). Psychometric validation of the Obsessive Belief Questionnaire and Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory—Part 2: Factor analyses and testing of a brief version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(11), 1527–1542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 93–115. doi:10.1037/a0022658.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purdon, C. (2001). Appraisal of obsessional thought recurrences: Impact on anxiety and mood state. Behavior Therapy, 32(1), 47–64. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80043-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Purdon, C. (2004). Empirical investigations of thought suppression in OCD. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35(2), 121–136. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.04.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (2000). White bears and other elusive intrusions assessing the relevance of thought suppression for obsessional phenomena. Behavior Modification, 24(3), 425–453. doi:10.1177/0145445500243008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purdon, C., Rowa, K., & Antony, M. M. (2005). Thought suppression and its effects on thought frequency, appraisal and mood state in individuals with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(1), 93–108. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.11.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. J. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(9), 793–802.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. J. (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions: Elaborations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(4), 385–401. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10041-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. J., & de Silva, P. (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16(4), 233–248.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radomsky, A. S., Alcolado, G. M., Abramowitz, J. S., Alonso, P., Belloch, A., Bouvard, M., et al. (2014). Part 1—You can run but you can’t hide: Intrusive thoughts on six continents. Journal of Obsessive–Compulsive and Related Disorders, 3(3), 269–279. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M. (1996). Cognitive-behavioral approaches to the understanding of obsessional problems. In R. M. Rapee (Ed.), Current controversies in the anxiety disorders (pp. 103–133). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M., & Harrison, J. (1984). Abnormal and normal obsessions: A replication. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22(5), 549–552. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(84)90057-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shafran, R., Thordarson, D. S., & Rachman, S. (1996). Thought-action fusion in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10(5), 379–391. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(96)00018-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprinkle, S. D., Lurie, D., Insko, S. L., Atkinson, G., Jones, G. L., Logan, A. R., & Bissada, N. N. (2002). Criterion validity, severity cut scores, and test-retest reliability of the Beck Depression Inventory-II in a university counseling center sample. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49(3), 381–385. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.49.3.381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steer, R. A., Ball, R., Ranieri, W. F., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Dimensions of the Beck Depression Inventory-II in clinically depressed outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 117–128. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199901)55:1<117:AID-JCLP12>3.0.CO;2-A.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steketee, G., Frost, R., & Bogart, K. (1996). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: Interview versus self-report. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(8), 675–684.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Hamlin, C., Foa, E. B., & Synodi, D. S. (2002a). Attributions for thought suppression failure in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26(4), 505–517. doi:10.1023/A:1016559117950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Przeworski, A., & Foa, E. B. (2002b). Thought suppression in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(11), 1255–1274. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00095-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolin, D. F., Woods, C. M., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2003). Relationship between obsessive beliefs and obsessive–compulsive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(6), 657–669. doi:10.1023/A:1026351711837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolin, D. F., Worhunsky, P., Brady, R. E., & Maltby, N. (2007). The relationship between obsessive beliefs and thought-control strategies in a clinical sample. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31(3), 307–318. doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9072-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolin, D. F., Worhunsky, P., & Maltby, N. (2006). Are “obsessive” beliefs specific to OCD?: A comparison across anxiety disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(4), 469–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101(1), 34–52. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1093–1104. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1093.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5–13. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A., & Davies, M. I. (1994). The Thought Control Questionnaire: A measure of individual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(8), 871–878. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)90168-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wheaton, M. G., Abramowitz, J. S., Berman, N. C., Riemann, B. C., & Hale, L. R. (2010). The relationship between obsessive beliefs and symptom dimensions in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 949–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Priya Balagopal for her assistance with this project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ryan J. Jacoby.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Ryan Jacoby, Rachel Leonard, Bradley Riemann, and Jonathan Abramowitz declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (national and institutional). Informed consent was obtained from all individual subjects participating in the study.

Animal Rights

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jacoby, R.J., Leonard, R.C., Riemann, B.C. et al. Self-Punishment as a Maladaptive Thought Control Strategy Mediates the Relationship Between Beliefs About Thoughts and Repugnant Obsessions. Cogn Ther Res 40, 179–187 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9741-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9741-1

Keywords

Navigation