Skip to main content
Log in

True and False Memories in Social Anxiety Disorder: Effects of Speech Anticipation and Social Content

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive paradigms allow clinical psychologists to examine memory processes, such as false memory production, to better understand psychopathology. The current study uses the Deese–Roediger–McDermott task to investigate true and false memories in a sample with social anxiety disorder (n = 37) compared to a non-anxious control group (n = 40) before and after a three-day delay following list presentation. Additionally, the study examines anticipation of a social stressor and stimuli content (social versus nonsocial) as moderators of memory effects. Contrary to hypotheses, results for true memories showed no effects involving social anxiety or stressor group. However, nonsocial false memories were reported more frequently when participants with social anxiety disorder were anticipating a speech and when control participants were not (the latter at the level of a trend). Notably, when lists were socially relevant, these group differences in false memory disappeared. Results suggest that individuals with social anxiety disorder may be vulnerable to some unexpected memory distortions when anticipating social stress.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Power analyses based on Wenzel et al. (2004) for the main group interaction effects, and Storbeck and Clore (2005) for the follow-up tests, indicate 80–90 % power with N = 80. Full details are available from the first author.

  2. The following kinds of words were excluded from the lists: 1) words that were extremely emotionally salient (e.g., “rape” for the date list), 2) words that were associates of non-social or non-outdoor homonyms (e.g., “calendar” for the date list), and 3) words that were very similar to a previously presented word (e.g., “celebration” and “celebrate” on the party list). In addition, words that had already been used as a stronger associate to a different list were excluded so that no words overlapped across lists.

  3. This study was part of a larger study examining information processing in social anxiety. For a full list of measures, please contact the first author.

  4. Inclusion instructions were used so that task demands would not exclude activated items that had been correctly identified as not on the lists. Checked lures indicate that the concept was activated, although the subjective sense of remembering did not occur. Unchecked lures reflect actual false memories.

References

  • Ashbaugh, A. R., Antony, M. M., McCabe, R. E., Schmidt, L. A., & Swinson, R. P. (2005). Self-evaluative biases in social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 387–398. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-2413-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 393–399. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Schnall, S. (2008). Affective coherence: Affect as embodied evidence in attitude, advertising, and art. In G. Semin & E. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 211–236). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cody, M. W., & Teachman, B. A. (2010). Post-event processing and memory bias for performance feedback in social anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 468–479. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.03.003.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cody, M. W., & Teachman, B. A. (2011). Global and local evaluations of public speaking performance in social anxiety. Behavior Therapy, 42, 601–611. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.01.004.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coles, M. E., & Heimberg, R. G. (2002). Memory biases in the anxiety disorders: Current status. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 587–627. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00113-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: Trait, state, and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 745–761. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00117-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355–391. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders, clinician version (SCID-CV). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P., Fossati, P., & Lepage, M. (2007). Modulation of memory formation by stimulus content: Specific role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the successful encoding of social pictures. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 351–362. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hege, A. C. G., & Dodson, C. S. (2004). Why distinctive information reduces false memories: Evidence for both impoverished relational-encoding and distinctiveness heuristic accounts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 787–795. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.787.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heimberg, R. G., & Becker, R. E. (2002). Cognitive-behavioral group therapy for social phobia: Basic mechanisms and clinical strategies. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. L. (2011). The adaptive nature of memory and its illusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 312–315. doi:10.1177/0963721411416571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, R. R., & McDaniel, M. A. (1993). The enigma of organization and distinctiveness. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 421–445. doi:10.1006/jmla.1993.1023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Teachman, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Sadder and less accurate? False memory for negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 412–417. doi:10.1037/a0015621.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kučera, F., & Francis, W. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 54–64. doi:10.1038/nrn1825.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeMoult, J., & Joormann, J. (2012). Attention and memory biases in social anxiety disorder: The role of comorbid depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36, 47–57. doi:10.1007/s10608-010-9322-2.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 455–470. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, C. L., Nelson, D. L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1177–1194. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.5.1177.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, J. (2010). Autobiographical memory biases in social anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 288–297. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.12.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1998). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Retrieved from http://www.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/.

  • Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Small, S. L., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes in the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1818–1829. doi:10.1162/0898929042947847.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, J. E., & Dodson, C. S. (2009). Investigating the mechanisms fuelling reduced false recall of emotional material. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 238–259. doi:10.1080/02699930801976663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapee, R. M., & Lim, L. (1992). Discrepancy between self- and observer ratings of performance in social phobics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 728–731.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L, I. I. I., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L, I. I. I., Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 385–407. doi:10.3758/BF03196177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 362, 773–786. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 296–303. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1403&4_20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., Luo, C. R., Kopecky, J. J., Price, C. A., Rothschild, L., Fung, N. S., & Schwartz, M. A. (2002). Are false memories more difficult to forget than accurate memories? The effect of retention interval on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1054–1064. doi:10.3758/BF03194323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinman, S. A., Gorlin, E. I., Teachman, B. A. (2014). Cognitive biases among individuals with social anxiety. In J. Weeks (Ed.), The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Social Anxiety Disorder. Chichester: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118653920.ch15.

  • Storbeck, J. (2013). Negative affect promotes encoding of and memory for details at the expense of the gist: Affect, encoding, and false memories. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 800–819. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.741060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory: Mood and the false memory effect. Psychological Science, 16, 785–791. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01615.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1191–1207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, A., Jostad, C., Brendle, J. R., Ferraro, F. R., & Lystad, C. M. (2004). An investigation of false memories in anxious and fearful individuals. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 32, 257–274. doi:10.1017/S1352465804001407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This project was conducted at the University of Virginia and facilitated by an R01AG033033 grant from the National Institute of Aging to Bethany Teachman. The authors would like to thank the Program for Anxiety, Cognition, and Treatment (PACT) lab, especially Deidra Childress, Katherine Fishbein, Melissa Gasser, Rachel Lazarus, Michelle Pasciuti, Elizabeth Stevens, and Alexandra Werntz for their valuable research assistance. In addition, thanks to Gerald Clore and Chad Dodson for their significant contributions to the design of this project and interpretation of results, and to Janet P. Trammell for her assistance with the methodology.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meghan W. Cody.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Meghan Cody, Shari Steinman, and Bethany Teachman declare that they have no conflict of interest relevant to the current study.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all participants for being included in the study.

Animal Rights

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 2.

Table 2 DRM word lists and critical lures (underlined)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cody, M.W., Steinman, S.A. & Teachman, B.A. True and False Memories in Social Anxiety Disorder: Effects of Speech Anticipation and Social Content. Cogn Ther Res 39, 797–807 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9712-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9712-6

Keywords

Navigation