Advertisement

Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 36, Issue 5, pp 441–450 | Cite as

Mindfulness is Inversely Associated with Alcohol Attentional Bias Among Recovering Alcohol-Dependent Adults

  • Eric L. GarlandEmail author
  • Charlotte A. Boettiger
  • Susan Gaylord
  • Vicki West Chanon
  • Matthew O. Howard
Original Article

Abstract

Although mindfulness has been linked with salutary clinical outcomes, less is known about its relation to cognitive mechanisms implicated in the onset and maintenance of alcohol dependence. Because trait mindfulness is associated with attentional control and emotion regulation, we hypothesized that trait mindfulness would be inversely associated with attentional bias towards visual alcohol cues. We tested this hypothesis in a sample of alcohol-dependent adults residing in a treatment facility, who completed questionnaires on trait mindfulness, craving, and stress, as well as a spatial cueing task designed to assess alcohol attentional bias. Recovering alcohol-dependent individuals high in trait mindfulness exhibited less alcohol attentional bias (AB), stress, and craving, and greater alcohol-related self-efficacy, than their counterparts low in trait mindfulness. Multiple linear regression analyses indicated that trait mindfulness was more predictive of alcohol AB than stress, craving, alcohol-related self-efficacy, time in treatment, or pre-treatment level of alcohol consumption. Identification of malleable traits that can offset automatic cognitive mechanisms implicated in addiction may prove to be crucial to treatment development efforts.

Keywords

Mindfulness Attentional bias Craving Stress Alcohol dependence 

Notes

Acknowledgments

ELG was supported by Grant Number T32AT003378 from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine, a Francisco J. Varela Research Grant from the Mind and Life Institute, and an Armfield-Reeves Innovation Grant from the UNC-Chapel Hill School of Social Work. CAB was supported by Award Number KL2RR025746 from the National Center for Research Resources. VWC was supported by Grant Number F32DA025442 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors acknowledge the expert technical assistance of Laura Andrews.

References

  1. Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T. M., Chawla, N., Simpson, T. L., Ostafin, B. D., et al. (2006). Mindfulness meditation and substance use in an incarcerated population. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(3), 343–347.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T. M., & Marlatt, G. A. (2007). The role of thought suppression in the relationship between mindfulness meditation and alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors, 32(10), 2324–2328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Breslin, F. C., Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., & Agrawal, S. (2000). A comparison of a brief and long version of the situational confidence questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 1211–1220.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123–152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal of Behavior Medicine, 31(1), 23–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carrasco, M. (2006). Covert attention increases contrast sensitivity: Psychophysical, neurophysiological, and neuroimaging studies. In S. Martinez-Conde, S. L. Macknik, L. M. Martinez, J. M. Alonso & T. U. Tse (Eds.), Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 154). doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)54003-8.
  8. Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 560–572.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201–215.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Duncan, J., Ward, R., & Shapiro, K. (1994). Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision. Nature, 369, 313–315.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Field, M. (2006). Attentional biases in drug abuse and addiction: Cognitive mechanisms, causes, consequences, and implications. In M. Munafo & I. Albery (Eds.), Cognition and addiction (pp. 73–99). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Field, M., & Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 97(1–2), 1–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Field, M., & Eastwood, B. (2005). Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 183(3), 350–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Field, M., Mogg, K., Zetteler, J., & Bradley, B. P. (2004). Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: the roles of initial orienting and maintained attention. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 176(1), 88–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Field, M., Munafo, M., & Franken, I. (2009). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 589–607.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Field, M., & Powell, H. (2007). Stress increases attentional bias for alcohol cues in social drinkers who drink to cope. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 42(6), 560–566.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Flannery, B. A., Volpicelli, J. R., & Pettinati, H. M. (1999). Psychometric properties of the Penn alcohol craving scale. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 23(8), 1289–1295.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Garland, E. L. (2007). The meaning of mindfulness: A second-order cybernetics of stress, metacognition, and coping. Complementary Health Practice Review, 12(1), 15–30.Google Scholar
  21. Garland, E. L., Boettiger, C. A., & Howard, M. O. (2011). Targeting cognitive-affective risk mechanisms in stress-precipitated alcohol dependence: An integrated, biopsychosocial model of allostasis, automaticity, and addiction. Medical Hypotheses, 76, 745–754.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Garland, E. L., Gaylord, S. A., Boettiger, C. A., & Howard, M. O. (2010). Mindfulness training modifies cognitive, affective, and physiological mechanisms implicated in alcohol dependence: Results from a randomized controlled pilot trial. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 42(2), 177–192.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Herndon, F. (2008). Testing mindfulness with perceptual and cognitive factors: External vs. internal encoding, and the cognitive failures questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(1), 32–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hopfinger, J. B., & Maxwell, J. S. (2005). Appearing and disappearing stimuli trigger a reflexive modulation of visual cortical activity. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 48–56.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jha, A., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Loas, G., Fremaux, D., Otmani, O., Lecercle, C., & Delahousse, J. (1997). Is alexithymia a negative factor for maintaining abstinence? A follow up study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 38(5), 296–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ludwig, D. S., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2008). Mindfulness in medicine. JAMA, 300(11), 1350–1352.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163–169.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Monti, P. M., Abrams, D. B., Kadden, R. M., & Cooney, N. L. (1989). Treating alcohol dependence: A coping skills training guide. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  30. Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness, and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 176–186.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Noel, X., Colmant, Van., Der Linden, M., Bechara, A., Bullens, Q., Hanak, C., et al. (2006). Time course of attention for alcohol cues in abstinent alcohol patients: The role of initial orienting. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 30(11), 1871–1877.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ostafin, B. D., & Marlatt, G. A. (2008). Surfing the urge: Experiential acceptance moderates the relation between automatic alcohol motivation and hazardous drinking. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27, 404–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention, self-regulation, and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 353, 1915–1927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sayette, M., Reichle, E. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Lost in the sauce: The effects of alcohol on mind wandering. Psychological Science, 20(6), 747–752.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schmertz, S. K., Anderson, P. L., & Robins, D. L. (2009). The relation between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of sustained attention. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31, 60–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric intervention for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22–33.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., et al. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biology, 5(6), e138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Soto, D., Hodsoll, J., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Automatic guidance of attention from working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 342–348.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Stormark, K. M., Field, N. P., Hugdahl, K., & Horowitz, M. (1997). Selective processing of visual alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholics: An approach-avoidance conflict? Addictive Behaviors, 22(4), 509–519.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., et al. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(43), 17152–17156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 83–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Theeuwes, J. (2005). Irrelevant singletons capture attention. In L. Itti, G. Rees, & J. K. Tsotsos (Eds.), Neurobiology of attention (pp. 418–424). London: Elsevier Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review, 97(2), 147–168.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Townshend, J. M., & Duka, T. (2007). Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli in alcohol-dependent inpatients. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 31(8), 1349–1357.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wiers, R. W., Houben, K., Smulders, F. T., Conrod, P. J., & Jones, B. T. (2006). To drink or not to drink: The role of automatic and controlled cognitive processes in the etiology of alcohol-related problems. In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 339–362). Sage: New York.Google Scholar
  46. Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 597–605.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Eric L. Garland
    • 1
    Email author
  • Charlotte A. Boettiger
    • 2
  • Susan Gaylord
    • 3
  • Vicki West Chanon
    • 4
  • Matthew O. Howard
    • 5
  1. 1.College of Social Work, Trinity Institute for the Addictions, Florida State UniversityTallahasseeUSA
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Biomedical Research Imaging Center, Bowles Center for Alcohol StudiesUniversity of North CarolinaChapel HillUSA
  3. 3.School of Medicine, Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation—Program on Integrative MedicineUniversity of North CarolinaChapel HillUSA
  4. 4.Department of Psychology, Behavioral Neuroscience ProgramChapel HillUSA
  5. 5.School of Social WorkUniversity of North CarolinaChapel HillUSA

Personalised recommendations