Skip to main content
Log in

Perceived Extent and Effectiveness of Reflection and Brooding in Relation to Depressed Mood

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The goal of this study was to determine whether the perceived effectiveness of two components of rumination, namely reflection and brooding, would differ, and whether these perceptions would interact with the extent that reflection and brooding were personally experienced, to predict depressed mood states. Participants completed measures of depressed mood, rumination, and measures of the personal and general effectiveness of rumination. Brooding was perceived to be less personally and generally effective compared to reflection. Depressed moods were predicted by high levels of brooding irrespective of effectiveness perceptions. High levels of reflection predicted depressed mood scores when the general effectiveness of reflection was perceived to be low. From a therapeutic perspective, these results suggest that brooding should be targeted irrespective of beliefs about the effectiveness of brooding but interventions designed to attenuate reflection should do so in the context of an individual’s beliefs about its general effectiveness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Sphericity could not be assumed, hence the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alfaraj, A., Spada, M. M., Nikcevic, A. V., Puffett, A., & Meer, S. (2009). Positive beliefs about rumination in depressed and non-depressed pregnant women: a preliminary investigation. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 27(1), 54–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allan, S., & Gilbert, P. (1995). A social-comparison scale—psychometric properties and relationship to psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences, 19(3), 293–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnhofer, T., Kuehn, E. M., de Jong-Meyer, R., & Williams, J. M. G. (2007). Beliefs about benefits of rumination in depressed men and women with and without a history of assault. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35(3), 317–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernblum, R., & Mor, N. (2010). Rumination and emotion-related biases in refreshing information. Emotion, 10(3), 423–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burwell, R. A., & Shirk, S. R. (2007). Subtypes of rumination in adolescence: Associations between brooding, reflection, depressive symptoms, and coping. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(1), 56–65.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, S., Miranda, R., & Surrence, K. (2009). Subtypes of rumination in the relationship between negative life events and suicidal ideation. Arch Suicide Res, 13(2), 123–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., & Williams, J. M. G. (2007). Reflection, brooding, and suicidality: A preliminary study of different types of rumination in individuals with a history of major depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 497–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daches, S., Mor, N., Winquist, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2010). Brooding and attentional control in processing self-encoded information: Evidence from a modified Garner task. Cognition & Emotion, 24(5), 876–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong-Meyer, R., Beck, B., & Riede, K. (2009). Relationships between rumination, worry, intolerance of uncertainty and metacognitive beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(4), 547–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Debeer, E., Hermans, D., & Raes, F. (2009). Associations between components of rumination and autobiographical memory specificity as measured by a minimal instructions autobiographical memory test. Memory, 17(8), 892–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, N. P., Papadakis, A. A., Hogan, C. M., & Strauman, T. J. (2009). Over and over again: Rumination, reflection, and promotion goal failure and their interactive effects on depressive symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(3), 254–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy, 37(3), 269–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking “why” does not hurt—Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16(9), 709–715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X. L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation-coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 172–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miranda, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Brooding and reflection: Rumination predicts suicidal ideation at 1-year follow-up in a community sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(12), 3088–3095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolenhoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective-study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster—the 1989 Loma-Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 115–121.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papageorgiou, C., & Siegle, G. J. (2003). Rumination and depression: Advances in theory and research. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 243–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2001). Metacognitive beliefs about rumination in recurrent major depression. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 8(2), 160–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2003). An empirical test of a clinical metacognitive model of rumination and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 261–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The center for epidemiologic studies depression scale a self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1991). The use of the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale in adolescents and young-adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20(2), 149–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J., Moore, P. M., & Thase, M. E. (2004). Rumination: One construct, many features in healthy individuals, depressed individuals, and individuals with lupus. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28(5), 645–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. M., & Alloy, L. B. (2009). A roadmap to rumination: A review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(2), 116–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surrence, K., Miranda, R., Marroquin, B. M., & Chan, S. (2009). Brooding and reflective rumination among suicide attempters: cognitive vulnerability to suicidal ideation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(9), 803–808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takano, K., & Tanno, Y. (2009). Self-rumination, self-reflection, and depression: Self-rumination counteracts the adaptive effect of self-reflection. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(3), 260–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thwaites, R., & Dagnan, D. (2004). Moderating variables in the relationship between social comparison and depression: An evolutionary perspective. Psychology and Psychotherapy-Theory Research and Practice, 77, 309–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination Reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. (2005). Positive beliefs about rumination in depression—a replication and extension. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(1), 73–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to P. A. Gooding.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gooding, P.A., Taylor, P.J. & Tarrier, N. Perceived Extent and Effectiveness of Reflection and Brooding in Relation to Depressed Mood. Cogn Ther Res 36, 282–289 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9367-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9367-x

Keywords

Navigation