Advertisement

Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 32, Issue 2, pp 261–280 | Cite as

Implementation Intentions Facilitate Response Inhibition in Children with ADHD

  • Caterina Gawrilow
  • Peter M. Gollwitzer
Original Article

Abstract

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is associated with action control problems such as failure to inhibit inappropriate responses. Two studies investigated whether self-regulation by implementation intentions (if-then plans; Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503) facilitates response inhibition in children with ADHD. In Study 1, children with ADHD who furnished a suppression goal with implementation intentions improved inhibition of an unwanted response on a Go/No-Go task to the same level observed in children without ADHD. Study 2 showed that a combination of implementation intentions and psychostimulant medication resulted in the highest level of suppression performance in children with ADHD. Theoretical and applied implications of these results are discussed.

Keywords

Children with ADHD Executive functions Response inhibition Self-regulation Implementation intentions 

References

  1. Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53–63PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child Behavior Checklist. (1998). Elternfragebogen über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen. Deutsche Bearbeitung der Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/4-18). Köln: Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik (KJFD). [German consensus version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/4-18)].Google Scholar
  3. Arnsten, A. F. T., & Dudley, A. G. (2005). Methylphenidate improves prefrontal cortical cognitive function through α2 adrenoceptor and dopamine D1 receptor actions: Relevance to therapeutic effects in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Behavioral Brain Functions, 1, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  5. Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  6. Bedard, A., Ickowicz, A., Logan, G. D., Hogg-Johnson, S., Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (2003). Selective inhibition in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder off and on stimulant medication. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 315–327.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Berlin, L., Bohlin, G., & Rydell, A. (2003). Relations between inhibition, executive functioning, and ADHD symptoms: A longitudinal study from age 5 to 8½ years. Child Neuropsychology, 9, 255–266.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B. L., Davenport, N. D., Li, W., Parrish, T. B., Gitelman, D. R., & Mesulam, M. M. (2005). Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 94–11.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brandstaetter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clarke, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2000). Do executive function deficits differentiate between adolescents with ADHD and Oppositional Defiant/Conduct Disorder? A neuropsychological study using the Six elements test and Hayling sentence completion test. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 403–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cohen, A., Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (in press). Implementation intentions and executive control: Task switch performance and the Simon effect. Psychological Research.Google Scholar
  12. Durston, S. (2003). A review of the biological bases of ADHD: What have we learned from imaging studies? Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9, 184–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2005). Executive functioning in hyperactive children as young adults: Attention, inhibition, response perseveration, and the impact of comorbidity. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 107–133.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frazier, T. W., Demaree, H. A., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2004). Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 18, 543–555.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. In W. Stroebe, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 141–185). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
  16. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). The volitional benefits of planning. In P. M. Gollwitzer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 287–312). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  17. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstaetter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gollwitzer, P. M., Bayer, U., & McCulloch, K. (2005). The control of the unwanted. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 485–515). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Groening, M. (Writer). (1990). The call of the Simpsons [Television series episode]. In J. L. Brooks, & S. Simon (Producer), The Simpsons. Los Angeles: Twentieth Century Fox.Google Scholar
  22. Halperin, J. M., & Schulz, K. P. (2006). Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 560–581.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hood, J., Baird, G., Rankin, P. M., & Isaacs, E. (2005) Immediate effects of methylphenidate on cognitive attention skills of children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, 408–414.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jensen, P. S. (2003). Comorbidity and child psychopathology: recommendations for the next decade. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 293–300.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., Greenhill, L. L., Conners, C. K., Arnold, L. E., Abikoff, H. B., Elliott, G., Hechtman, L., Hoza, B., March, J. S., Newcorn, J. H., Severe, J. B., Vitiello, B., Wells, K., & Wigal, T. (2001). Findings from the NIMH Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD (MTA): Implications and applications for primary care providers. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 60–73.Google Scholar
  26. Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91, 295–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 1065–1081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mehta, M. A., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Mavaddat, N., Pickard, J. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete fornatl and parietal lobe regions in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1–6.Google Scholar
  30. Meichenbaum, D. H., & Goodman, J. (1971). Training impulsive children to talk to themselves: A means of developing self-control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 115–126.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Midha, K. K., McKay, G., Rawson, M. J., Korchinski, E. D., & Hubbard, J. W. (2001). Effects of food on the pharmacokinetics of Methylphenidate. Pharmaceutical Research, 18, 1185–1189.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 621–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Nichols, S. L., & Waschbusch, D. A. (2004). A review of the validity of laboratory cognitive tasks used to asses symptoms of ADHD. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 34, 297–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Oosterlaan, J., Logan, G. D., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Response inhibition in ADHD, CD, comorbid ADHD + CD, anxious and normal children: a meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 411–425.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1996). Inhibition in ADHD, aggressive, and anxious children: A biologically based model of child psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 19–36.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Overtoom, C. C., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., Kemner, C., van der Molen, M. W., van Engeland, H., Buitelaar, J. K., & Koelega, H. S. (2002). Inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A psychophysiological study of the stop task. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 668–676.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 945–954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Owens, J. S., & Hoza, B. (2003). The role of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in the positive illusory bias. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 680–691.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Parks-Stamm, E., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Action control by implementation intentions: Effective cue detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, 25, 248–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O., & Roberts, R. J. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In G. R. Lyon, & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327–348). Baltimore, MD, US: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  42. Pliszka, S. R., Glahn, D. C., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Franklin, C., Perez, R., Xiong, J., & Liotti, M. (2006). Neuroimaging of inhibitory control areas in children with ADHD who were treatment naive or in long-term treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1052–1060.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Pliszka, S. R., Liotti, M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2000). Inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: event-related potential identify the processing component and timing of an impaired right-frontal response-inhibition mechanism. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 238–246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Pliszka, S. R., Borcherding, S. H., Spratley, K., Leon, S., & Irick S. (1997). Measuring inhibitory control in children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 18, 254–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Posner, M. L, & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development & Psychpathology, 12, 427–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Reid, R., Trout, A. L., & Schartz, M. (2005). Self-regulation interventions for children with ADHD. Exceptional Children, 71, 361–377.Google Scholar
  47. Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 343–350.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., Geurts, H., Morein-Zamir, S., Meiran, N., Schut, H., Vlasveld, L., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). Executive functioning in boys with ADHD: Primarily an inhibition deficit? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 569–594.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2000). The construct of goal-commitment: Measurement and relationships with task performance. In R. Goffin, & E. Helmes (Eds.), Problems and solutions in human assessment (pp. 315–332). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
  50. Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intentions and repeated behavior: Augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of planned behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 349–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between goal intentions and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Smith, A., & Taylor, E. (2006). Response inhibition and hyperactivity in clinical, non-clinical populations: A meta-analysis using the stop task. In R. D. Oades (Ed.), AD/HD and HKS: Current ideas and ways forward (pp. 203–225). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
  53. Smith, A., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Toone, B., & Rubia, K. (2006). Task-specific hypoactivation in prefrontal and temporoparietal brain regions during motor inhibition and task switching in medication-naïve children and adolescents with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1044–1051.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD-a dual pathway model of behavior and cognition. Behavioral Brain Research, 130, 29–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 65–99.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. The World Medical Association (2004). World Medical Association declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Retrieved June 3, 2006 from http://www.wma.net/e/policy/b3.htm.Google Scholar
  57. Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions help to overcome ego-depletion? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 279–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2007). How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 295–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. WHO (1991). Internationale Klassifikation psychischer Stoerungen: ICD 10, Kapitel V. Bern: Huber. [Tenth revision of the International classification of diseases, Chapter V (F): Mental and behavioral disorders].Google Scholar
  60. Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 35–78.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wu, K. K., Anderson, V., & Castiello, U. (2006). ADHD and working memory: A task switching paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 1288–1306.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 71–93). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
  63. Zhu, J. (2004). Locating volition. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 302–322.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of KonstanzKonstanzGermany
  2. 2.Psychology DepartmentNew York UniversityNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations