Study 1: The Effect of Car Size on Driving Behaviour
In this study, we assess behavioural evidence of a positive effect of car size on risk taking in a driving context. We collaborated with a Belgian non-profit that operates a full-scale, realistic driving simulator developed by Green DinoFootnote 1 for driver training. We manipulated the size of the car that participants believed to be driving. In reality, we set the simulator with identical parameters between conditions. The dependent measure was a composite measure of driving intensity—commonly related to crash risk (Eboli et al., 2017). We measured regulatory focus to assess whether the effect of car size on risk taking only occurs at low levels of promotion and prevention scores (H3). Additionally, we measured mood, driving experience, and attitude towards driving as covariates of our dependent measure driving intensity. Mood can influence evaluations of risk (Forgas, 1995), and we expected driving experience and attitude to make participants more risk tolerant. Considering the limited attainable sample size that results from the operational limitations of working with a car simulator, we collected these covariates to reduce error variance and obtain a more accurate measurement of our hypothesized effects (Meyvis & Van Osselaer, 2018).
Material and Procedure
Participants (n = 49, 23 female, ages 18–23) were undergraduate students from a large European university, part of a university subject pool for behavioural studies. They came individually to a dedicated room at the university for a 20-min session in return for a monetary compensation of 8€. Candidates needed a valid driver’s license to participate. While the complex experimental procedure and limited availability of only a single driving simulator prevented us from collecting a larger sample of data, a power analysis showed adequate power between 81.5% and 99.4% (with α = 0.05) for the analyses below.
First, participants learned about the procedure on a computer screen. They were told that we were studying people’s driving behaviour in response to specific circumstances on the road. We assessed participants’ driving experience and attitude and measured participants’ regulatory focus as two separate scales of promotion and prevention focus. We also assessed participants’ mood based on the PANAS (Watson et al., 1988). We then explained that the rest of this study would take place in the driving simulator, and participants were at that point randomly assigned to the car size condition. They were shown a picture of the car they would drive with the model name mentioned, which was either a big car (a Toyota Avensis wagon) or a small car (a Toyota Yaris). Brand was deliberately kept constant and consistent with the layout of the interior of the simulator, which showed the brand logo placed centrally on the steering wheel. The last computer screen then invited participants to take place in the driving simulator. The simulator asked participants to drive naturally throughout the session. Participants were first allowed a two-min practice drive. They were then instructed to drive the car for 10 min along a predetermined route. An average traffic density was simulated, including other cars, pedestrians, and cyclists. During this second drive, the simulator measured speed in kilometres per hour, acceleration and deceleration both as kilometre per hour squared, and throttle and braking as percentage of full pedal articulation, all sampled at five measurements per second. After each ride, the simulation software provided averages of these measures that constitute our dependent variables.
Results and Discussion
Because some of the driving parameters (e.g., speed, in km/h) were a magnitude bigger than others (e.g., braking, in percentage of maximum braking pedal articulation), we standardized all items before averaging them into a single measure of driving intensity (Cronbach’s α = 0.90). Neither mood, driving experiences, nor attitude led to any significant effects. Hence, we did not include these variables for further analysis.
A first analysis revealed that the bigger car led to a higher driving intensity (M = 0.23, SE = 0.16) than the smaller car (M = -0.28, SE = 0.16; t(47) = 2.26, p = 0.023). We found significant differences for most individual variables included in driving intensity, except average speed and braking, which show directional effects (Table 1).
Table 1 Independent samples t tests for driving parameters between conditions The two subscales of regulatory focus—prevention and promotion—did correlate with each other, but only weakly (r = − 0.31, p = 0.028), and testing internal consistency with reversed prevention items did not warrant aggregating all items into a single score (Cronbach’s α = 0.37), so we used the subscales separately for the remainder of the analysis. A general linear model analysis of the data yielded no three-way interaction between car type, prevention, and promotion (F < 1, ns.). Analysing the effects of promotion and prevention separately, we did find two significant two-way interactions that qualified the main effect of car type on driving intensity.
First, we found a significant interaction between car type and promotion focus on driving intensity (F(1,45) = 4.36, p = 0.042). Only participants low in promotion focus were sensitive to the effect of car type. To examine this interaction in more detail, we conducted a spotlight analysis (Fitzsimons, 2008; Irwin & McClelland, 2001). At one standard deviation above the mean of promotion, we found no significant difference in driving intensity between participants in the small car and those in the big car (Msmall = 0.23 and Mbig = 0.19; β = − 0.05, SE = 0.32, t(45) = − 0.14, p = 0.89). At one standard deviation below the mean of promotion, however, participants in the big car seemed to drive more intensively (Msmall = − 0.69 and Mbig = 0.29; β = 0.99, SE = 0.31, t(45) = 3.20, p = 0.0025). Participants high in promotion focus drive with high intensity, no matter what car type.
Mirroring the previous result, we found a near significant interaction effect between car type and prevention focus (F(1, 45) = 3.09, p = 0.086). Again, we used spotlight analysis to find that at one standard deviation above the mean of prevention, no significant difference appeared in driving behaviour between participants in the small car and those in the big car (Msmall = − 0.20 and Mbig = 0.02; β = 0.23, SE = 0.32, t(45) = 0.70, p = 0.49). At one standard deviation below the mean of prevention, however, participants in the big car seemed to drive more intensively (Msmall = − 0.33 and Mbig = 0.49; β = 0.83, SE = 0.32, t(45) = 2.55, p = 0.014). Only participants that scored low on prevention focus seemed to be affected by car type. Participants with high prevention focus drove cautiously, no matter what car they believed to be driving (Fig. 1).
When participants believe to be driving a bigger car, this leads to more intense and risky driving behaviour. These results provide initial support for the car cushion hypothesis. Moreover, our results support that promotion and prevention focus can impose boundary conditions on this hypothesis, in that consumers scoring high on either promotion or prevention focus are less susceptible to the context effect on which the car cushion hypothesis hinges.
Study 2: The Effect of Car Size on Generalized Risk Taking
Study 2 aimed at replicating and extending the effect of car size on risk taking beyond driving. Thus, we seek to find support for the notion that bigger cars encourage generalized risk taking. We set up this second study as a scenario study with behavioural and consequential outcomes, facilitating a bigger sample size to ensure robustness of findings.
Material and Procedure
Participants (n = 214, 126 female, ages 18–27) were subject pool members of the behavioural lab of a large European university who came to our behavioural lab in groups of eight and participated in return for a monetary base payment of 6€. Candidates needed a valid driver’s license to participate. Our manipulation comprised having participants evaluate one of two car ads, randomly assigned between participants. We asked participants to imagine they were in the market for a new car, and asked to imagine considering the presented car as a candidate choice (Peck et al., 2013). Both cars were from the same brand to control for brand-related associations. The big car condition showed a Mercedes R-class belonging to the MPV category, while the small car condition showed a Mercedes A-class belonging to the compact category. Despite their substantial difference in size, these cars have several design elements in common. We selected four imagesFootnote 2 for each car, of which three smaller ones from the press kit showing the car from a front-side angle, from the back, and from the side. For each condition, we added one larger image showing the car from a front-side angle, driving, with visible people inside for size reference. As part of the manipulation, we gave a short description of each car, including for the R-class that it was the “largest in its class for all your transportation needs,” and for the A-class that it was “small and nimble for navigating the city.” We then asked participants to report their perception of the car on four items (Safety, Build quality, Price, Performance), each rated on 7-point Likert-type scales. After rating the car, we told participants this part of the session was completed and asked them to move to the next—ostensibly unrelated—task comprising our dependent measure.
As a dependent measure, we used the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002). Among attitudinal and behavioural measures of risk taking, the BART has demonstrated itself to have high predictive validity for risk taking in natural settings, including drug use, unprotected sex, gambling, and stealing (Fox & Tannenbaum, 2011; Lauriola et al., 2014; Lejuez et al., 2002). In the BART, participants receive 5 cents for every time they push the button that pumps a balloon further but lose all of their money on a specific balloon if it pops. Thus, every additional pump increases the total gain but also increases the risk of loss on a trial. Participants can cash out on a balloon if they do not want to take extra risk or give an extra pump to increase their outcome with the risk of popping the balloon. Participants repeated this task over 20 independent balloons. We ensured incentive compatibility by randomly drawing one participant per session who won the money made in the task.
Results and Discussion
Following instructions on interpreting the BART, we calculated the corrected average number of pumps for each participant, which is the average number of pumps across balloons, excluding balloons that popped (Lejuez et al., 2002). Car size affected the corrected average number of pumps (Msmall = 14.83 and Mbig = 16.16, t(212) = 3.52, p < 0.001). Perceptions of the cars differed significantly with the bigger car scoring higher on all dimensions (see Table 2). To account for potential response bias, we regressed the corrected average number of pumps on condition and the four perception dimensions. Only condition (β = 0.17, t(208) = 2.19, p = 0.029) and perceived safety (β = 0.18, t(208) = 2.16, p = 0.032) remained significant when including all these predictors.
Table 2 Independent sample t tests for car perceptions We tested whether perceived safety mediated the effect of car size on risk taking. A bootstrapping algorithm (PROCESS model 4; Hayes, 2017) supported partial mediation, with a significant indirect effect (M = 0.41, SE = 0.18, 95%CI [0.055, 77]) and significant direct effect (M = 0.93, SE = 0.41, 95%CI [0.12, 1.74]). We found no support for a similar mediating role of other dimensions. These results highlight the “cushion” aspect of our theorizing, in that perceived car safety stands out as best predicting generalized risk taking.