Skip to main content
Log in

Making Small Numbers Count: Environmental and Financial Feedback in Promoting Eco-driving Behaviours

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Consumer Policy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Energy conservation results in environmental (reduced emissions) and financial (reduced costs) savings. Consumers’ perception of the worthiness of changes in behaviour may differ depending on whether environmental or financial savings are emphasized. The current study investigated the effects of using either environmental or financial feedback in the context of eco-driving. Participants evaluated six scenarios describing different eco-driving behaviours. Participants in experimental groups were informed about either the environmental or financial savings realized by adopting the behaviours. A control group did not receive information on possible savings. Results indicated that, unlike commonly assumed, environmental savings are considered more worthwhile than commensurate financial savings. Yet, intentions to adopt eco-driving behaviours were mainly sensitive to the presence of feedback per se, rather than the content of feedback. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these findings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Financial savings were calculated based on oil price per litre in the Netherlands at the time of the study.

  2. A second between-subjects independent variable in the current study was time span of savings. We did not observe differences between framing savings in the short and long terms. We therefore collapsed the data across these groups.

  3. We could not establish whether feedback type influenced intention via worthiness as our data do not support an important precondition to establish mediation: the relationship between feedback type (environmental vs. financial) and intention was not significant for all behaviours.

References

  • Abrahamse, W., Steg, L., Vlek, C., & Rothengatter, T. (2005). A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 273–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birrell, S. A., & Young, M. S. (2011). The impact of smart driving aids on driving performance and driver distraction. Transportation Research Part F, 14, 484–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolderdijk, J. W., Steg, L., Geller, E. S., Lehman, P. K., & Postmes, T. (2013). Comparing the effectiveness of monetary versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nature Climate Change, 3, 413–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CIECA. (2007). Eco-driving in category B driver training and the driver testing. http://www.thepep.org/ClearingHouse/docfiles/CIECA.internal.project.on.Eco-driving.pdf. Accessed 24 Oct 2010.

  • Diekmann, A., & Preisendörfer, P. (2003). Green and greenback: the behavioral effects of environmental attitudes in low-cost and high-cost situations. Rationality and Society, 15, 441–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dietz, T., Gardner, G. T., Gilligan, J., Stern, P. C., & Vandenbergh, M. P. (2009). Household actions can provide a behavioral wedge to rapidly reduce US carbon emissions. Proceedings of the Naitonal Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 1061–18456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dogan, E., Steg, L., & Delhomme, P. (2011). The influence of multiple goals on driving behavior: the case of safety, time saving, and fuel saving. Accident analysis and Prevention, 43, 1635–1643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eco-drive Netherlands. (2011). How to eco-drive? http://www.ecodrivingnederland.nl/nl-NL/tips.aspx. Accessed 10 Feb 2012.

  • Fehr, E., & Falk, A. (2002). Psychological foundations of incentives. European Economic Review, 46, 687–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, C. (2008). Feedback on household electricity consumption: A tool for saving energy? Energy Efficiency, 1, 79–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or don’t pay at all. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115, 791–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyman, J., & Ariely, D. (2004). Effort for payment: A tale of two markets. Psychological Science, 15, 787–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., & Rottenstreich, Y. (2004). Music, pandas, and muggers: On the affective psychology of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 23–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J., Blank, A., & Chanowitz, B. (1978). The mindlessness of ostensibly thoughtful action: the role of “placebic” information in interpersonal interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 635–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2007). Normative, gain and hedonic goal frames guiding environmental behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 65, 117–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54, 1053–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., Baldwin, A. S., Hertel, A. W., & Fuglestad, P. T. (2004). Self-regulation and behavior change: Disentangling behavioral initiation and behavioral maintenance. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications (pp. 106–122). New York: Guilford Press.

  • Siero, S., Boon, M., Kok, G., & Siero, F. (1989). Modification of driving behavior in a large transport organization: a field experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 417–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G., & Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and callousness: the impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 143–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Netherlands. (2011). Gemiddelde jaarkilometrage van personenauto’s (Average annual mileage of passenger cars). http://statline.cbs.nl/StatWeb/publication/?VW=T&DM=SLNL&PA=71107ned&D1=0&D2=a&D3=a&D4=0&D5=0&D6=a&HD=100329. Accessed 15 Oct 2012.

  • Stillwater, T., & Kurani, K. S. (2013). Drivers discuss ecodriving feedback: Goal setting, framing, and anchoring motivate new behaviours. Transportation Research Pat F, 19, 85–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrunsel, A. E. (1999). Trust as an obstacle in environmental-economic disputes. American Behavioral Scientist, 42, 1350–1367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • The Daily Green. (2011). Ten ways to save money on gas. http://www.thedailygreen.com/environmental-news/latest/save-gas-47031702#ixzz1FSNveFDM. Accessed 25 March 2011.

  • Thøgersen, J., & Crompton, T. (2009). Simple and painless? The limitations of spillover in environmental campaigning. Journal of Consumer Policy, 32, 141–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (2005). Emission facts: average CO2-emissions resulting from gasoline and diesel fuel (report no: EPA420-F-05-001). http://www.carbonsolutions.com/Resources/Average_Carbon_Dioxide_Emissions_Resulting_from_Gasoline_and_Diesel_Fuel.pdf. Accessed 25 March 2011.

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., Matos, L., & Lacante, M. (2004). Less is sometimes more: Goal content matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 755–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ebru Dogan.

Appendix

Appendix

Avoiding idling

 As you approach a level crossing in your car, you hear the bell signalling that a train is approaching. The level crossing will be closed for a minute. If you turn off the engine of your car while you are waiting, you would save 0.018 kg CO 2 emissions/€ 0.05.

Speed reduction

 You are driving on a motorway with a 120-km/h speed limit. You still have 100 km to reach your destination. If you drive 100 km/h instead of 120 km/h, you would save 2.40 kg CO 2 emissions/€ 1.70 for this trip.

Overtaking

 You are driving on a motorway with a 120-km/h speed limit. You still have to travel 80 km to reach your destination. The cars in front of you drive between 100 and 110 km/h. There are opportunities to safely overtake. If you do not overtake the cars in front of you, you would save 1.92 kg CO 2 emissions/€ 1.35 for this trip.

Air conditioning

 You are travelling by your car on a warm day. You still have 40 km to go. You consider turning on the air conditioner. If you keep the air conditioner off, you would save 0.56 kg of CO 2 emissions/€ 0.44 for this trip.

Roof rack

 You have a roof rack installed on your car. You will need the roof rack again in a month. You consider keeping the roof rack on your car during this month. If you remove the roof rack now and re-install it when you need it, you would save 1.96 kg CO 2 emission/€ 1.20 during this month.

Tyre pressure

 You have not checked your tyres for some time and notice your tyres are underinflated. If you check the tyre pressure of your vehicle regularly on a monthly basis from now on, you would save 1.47 kg of CO 2 emissions/€ 0.90 over a month.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dogan, E., Bolderdijk, J.W. & Steg, L. Making Small Numbers Count: Environmental and Financial Feedback in Promoting Eco-driving Behaviours. J Consum Policy 37, 413–422 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10603-014-9259-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10603-014-9259-z

Keywords

Navigation