Skip to main content

Epistemic engagement: examining personal epistemology and engagement preferences with climate change in Oregon

Abstract

Engaging politically polarized publics surrounding climate science is a vital element in the effort to enact climate mitigation policy. Science communication experts have identified several models of public engagement with science, including the deficit, dialogue, participation, and lay expertise model. Existing research suggests that the deficit model in particular is a largely ineffective model of engagement for controversial science like climate change. There is very little research, however, regarding the engagement preferences of political groups, or how those preferences differ. This study assesses preferences for climate change engagement in the state of Oregon in the United States and examines the relationship between those preferences and epistemic beliefs about climate science. Overall, we find that liberals are significantly more likely than moderates or conservatives to view climate science as certain and simple and to rely on expert knowledge more than their own direct experience. By contrast, conservatives are significantly more likely than liberals or moderates to view climate science as uncertain and complex and to rely on their own direct experience over the knowledge of content experts. We also find that perceived certainty and simplicity are positive predictors of a preference for the deficit model of science communication. Implications for public engagement with climate change and suggestions for future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Figure 1
Figure 2
Figure 3
Figure 4
Figure 5

Notes

  1. 1.

    Importantly, these models of public engagement with science focus exclusively on the relationship between scientists and lay publics. These models do not aim to capture the broad and heterogeneous science communication environment that exists beyond these two groups.

  2. 2.

    For an in-depth review and discussion of domain specificity and domain generality, see Muis, Bendixen, and Haerle (2006).

  3. 3.

    Survey items used to measure epistemic complexity are similar to climate change consensus beliefs. To assess covariance between these two variables and ensure our operationalization of complexity is distinct from consensus perceptions, we ran a correlation analysis between the complexity index and two measures of consensus that were used within the same survey effort. Consensus perceptions were measured by asking participants what percentage of climate scientists agree that climate change is happening, and what percentage agree that climate change is human caused. We found that the complexity index is significantly and negatively correlated with both measures of consensus (r=-0.197, p<0.001; r=−0.228, p<0.001, respectively), such that higher complexity beliefs were associated with lower consensus perceptions. However, the shared variance of these variables (indicated by r2) demonstrates little covariance, indicating that complexity perceptions are distinct from consensus beliefs.

  4. 4.

    The models used capture response location dependence with location-specific random effects using the zip code of the latest address of residence from each respondent. We model the responses using multivariate linear models. In order to assess the uncertainty in the parameter estimates resulting from the model, we take a Bayesian approach and models were fitted using the rstan package in R.

  5. 5.

    This model accounts for location and within-subject dependence.

References

  1. Abramowitz AI, Saunders KL (2006) Exploring the bases of partisanship in the American electorate: social identity vs Ideology. Polit Res Q 59(2):175–187. https://doi.org/10.1177/106591290605900201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Bell L (2008) Engaging the public in technology policy: a new role for science museums. Sci Commun 29(3):386–398. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547007311971

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Bolsen T, Druckman JN, Cook FL (2014) The influence of partisan motivated reasoning on public opinion. Polit Behav 36(2):235–262. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9238-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Cameron F, Deslandes A (2011) Museums and science centres as sites for deliberative democracy on climate change. Museum Soc 9(2):136–153

    Google Scholar 

  5. Chirumbolo A, Areni A, Sensales G (2004) Need for cognitive closure and politics: voting, political attitudes and attributional style. Int J Psychol 39(4):245–253. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590444000005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Conley AM, Pintrich PR, Vekiri I, Harrison D (2004) Changes in epistemological beliefs in elementary science students. Contemp Educ Psychol 29(2):186–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.01.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Dawson E (2014) “Not designed for us”: how science museums and science centers socially exclude low-income, minority ethnic groups. Sci Educ 98(6):981–1008. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Druckman JN, McGrath MC (2019) The evidence for motivated reasoning in climate change preference formation. Nat Clim Chang 9(2):111–119. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-018-0360-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Dunlap RE, McCright A, Yarosh J (2016) The political divide on climate change: partisan polarization widens in the U.S. Environ Sci Policy Sustain Dev 58(5):4–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/00139157.2016.1208995

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Egan PJ, Mullin M (2017) Climate change: US public opinion. Annu Rev Polit Sci 20(1):209–227. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-051215-022857

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Eigenbrode SD, O’rourke M, Wulfhorst JD, Althoff DM, Goldberg CS, Merrill K, Morse W, Nielsen-Pincus M, Stephens J, Winowiecki L, Bosque-Pérez NA (2007) Employing philosophical dialogue in collaborative science. BioScience 57(1):55–64. https://doi.org/10.1641/B570109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Engineering National Academies of Sciences. (2016). Communicating science effectively: a research agenda. https://doi.org/10.17226/23674

  13. Federico CM, Deason G, Fisher EL (2012) Ideological asymmetry in the relationship between epistemic motivation and political attitudes. J Pers Soc Psychol 103(3):381–398 http://dx.doi.org.proxy.lib.pdx.edu/10.1037/a0029063. Accessed 3 Feb 2020

  14. Gallup. (2014). Oregon scorecard. Gallup Inc. https://news.gallup.com/poll/174737/oregon-scorecard.aspx. Accessed 28 Mar 2020

  15. Gauchat G (2011) The cultural authority of science: public trust and acceptance of organized science. Public Underst Sci 20(6):751–770. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662510365246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gauchat G (2012) Politicization of science in the public sphere: a study of public trust in the United States, 1974-2010. Am Sociol Rev 77(2):167–187

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hofer BK (2000) Dimensionality and disciplinary differences in personal epistemology. Contemp Educ Psychol 25(4):378–405. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Jost JT, Federico CM, Napier JL (2009) Political ideology: its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annu Rev Psychol 60(1):307–337. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163600

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Jost JT, Kruglanski A, & Simon L (2013). Effects of epistemic motivation on conservatism, intolerance, and other system-justifying attitudes. In Levine JM, Thompson LL, & Messick DM (Eds.), Shared Cognition in Organizations: The Management of Knowledge (pp. 91–116). Psychology Press.

  20. Kahan DM (2012). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection: an experimental study (SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 2182588). Social Science Research Network. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2182588. Accessed 13 Feb 2020

  21. Kahan DM, Peters E, Wittlin M, Slovic P, Ouellette LL, Braman D, Mandel G (2012) The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nat Clim Chang 2(10):732–735. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1547

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Kalmoe NP (2020) Uses and abuses of ideology in political psychology. Polit Psychol 41(4):771–793. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12650

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kampa N, Neumann I, Heitmann P, Kremer K (2016) Epistemological beliefs in science—a person-centered approach to investigate high school students’ profiles. Contemp Educ Psychol 46:81–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.04.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Kaplowitz MD, Lupi F (2012) Stakeholder preferences for best management practices for non-point source pollution and stormwater control. Landsc Urban Plan 104(3):364–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.11.013

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. King PM, Kitchener KS (2004) Reflective judgment: theory and research on the development of epistemic assumptions through adulthood. Educ Psychol 39(1):5–18. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3901_2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ko H (2016) In science communication, why does the idea of a public deficit always return? How do the shifting information flows in healthcare affect the deficit model of science communication? - Henry Ko, 2016. Public Underst Sci 25(4):427–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kruglanski AW, Pierro A, Mannetti L, De Grada E (2006) Groups as epistemic providers: need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. Psychol Rev 113(1):84–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Lazer DMJ, Baum MA, Benkler Y, Berinsky AJ, Greenhill KM, Menczer F, Metzger MJ, Nyhan B, Pennycook G, Rothschild D, Schudson M, Sloman SA, Sunstein CR, Thorson EA, Watts DJ, Zittrain JL (2018) The science of fake news. Science 359(6380):1094–1096. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aao2998

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Leiserowitz A (2006) Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: the role of affect, imagery, and values. Clim Chang 77(1):45–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-006-9059-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Levendusky M (2013a). How partisan media polarize America. University of Chicago Press.

  31. Levendusky MS (2013b) Why do partisan media polarize viewers? Am J Polit Sci 57(3):611–623. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Mason L (2018) Ideologues without issues: the polarizing consequences of ideological identities. Public Opin Q 82(S1):866–887. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfy005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Merkley E (2020) Anti-intellectualism, populism, and motivated resistance to expert consensus. Public Opin Q 84(1):24–48. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfz053

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Merkley E, Stecula DA (2018) Party elites or manufactured doubt? The informational context of climate change polarization. Sci Commun 40(2):258–274. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547018760334

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Merkley E, Stecula DA (2020) Party cues in the news: democratic elites, republican backlash, and the dynamics of climate skepticism. Br J Polit Sci:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123420000113

  36. Miller S (2010). Deficit model. Encycl Sci Technol Commun 208–210.

  37. Motta M, Stecula D, Farhart C (2020) How right-leaning media coverage of COVID-19 facilitated the spread of misinformation in the early stages of the pandemic in the U.S. Cann J Polit Sci/Revue Canadienne de Science Politique 53(2):335–342. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0008423920000396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Muis KR, Bendixen LD, Haerle FC (2006) Domain-generality and domain-specificity in personal epistemology research: philosophical and empirical reflections in the development of a theoretical framework. Educ Psychol Rev 18(1):3–54. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9003-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Nilsson A, von Borgstede C, Biel A (2004) Willingness to accept climate change strategies: the effect of values and norms. J Environ Psychol 24(3):267–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.06.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Oregon Secretary of State. (2019). Voter registrations and election participation (election statistics). Oregon Secretary of State. https://sos.oregon.gov/elections/Pages/electionsstatistics.aspx. Accessed 28 Mar 2020

  41. Oreskes N, & Conway EM (2011). Merchants of doubt: how a handful of scientists obscured the truth on issues from tobacco smoke to global warming. A&C Black.

  42. O’Rourke M, Crowley S, Eigenbrode S, & Wulfhorst JD (Eds.). (2014). Enhancing communication and collaboration in interdisciplinary research. SAGE.

  43. O’Rourke M, Crowley SJ (2013) Philosophical intervention and cross-disciplinary science: the story of the Toolbox Project. Synthese 190(11):1937–1954. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-012-0175-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Peer, J., & Lourdusamy, A. (2005). Students’ epistemological beliefs about science: the impact of school science experience. https://repository.nie.edu.sg//handle/10497/15472. Accessed 12 Jul 2019

  45. Peter J, Rosman T, Mayer A-K, Leichner N, Krampen G (2016) Assessing epistemic sophistication by considering domain-specific absolute and multiplicistic beliefs separately. Br J Educ Psychol 86(2):204–221. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12098

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Pew Research Center. (2017). Political typology reveals deep fissures on the right and left. http://assets.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/5/2017/10/31115611/10-24-2017-Typology-release.pdf. Accessed 23 May 2018

  47. Pidgeon N, Rogers-Hayden T (2007) Opening up nanotechnology dialogue with the publics: risk communication or ‘upstream engagement’? Health. Risk Soc 9(2):191–210. https://doi.org/10.1080/13698570701306906

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Pintrich PR (2002). Future challenges and directions for theory and research on personal epistemology. In Personal epistemology: the psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 389–414). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

  49. Prior L (2003) Belief, knowledge and expertise: the emergence of the lay expert in medical sociology. Sociol Health Illness 25(3):41–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.00339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Shtulman A (2013) Epistemic similarities between students’ scientific and supernatural beliefs. J Educ Psychol 105(1):199–212. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Simis MJ, Madden H, Cacciatore MA, Yeo SK (2016) The lure of rationality: why does the deficit model persist in science communication? Public Underst Sci 25(4):400–414. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662516629749

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Smith JW, Bitsura-Meszaros K, Keane R (2016) Differences between conservatives and liberals in information-seeking behavior and perceived risks associated with climate-driven changes to local forest conditions. Weather, Clim Soc 8(1):43–55. https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-15-0046.1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Steup M (2018). Epistemology. In Zalta EN (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (winter 2018). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2018/entries/epistemology/. Accessed 12 Jul 2019

  54. Strager MP, Rosenberger RS (2006) Incorporating stakeholder preferences for land conservation: weights and measures in spatial MCA. Ecol Econ 57(4):627–639. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2005.05.015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Suldovsky B (2016) In science communication, why does the idea of the public deficit always return? Exploring key influences. Public Underst Sci 25(4):415–426. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662516629750

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Suldovsky B, Landrum A, & Stroud NJ (2019). Public perceptions of who counts as a scientist for controversial science. Public Understand Sci 0963662519856768. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662519856768

  57. Suldovsky B, McGreavy B, Lindenfeld L (2017) Science communication and stakeholder expertise: insights from sustainability science. Environ Commun 11(5):587–592. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2017.1308408

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Suldovsky B, McGreavy B, Lindenfeld L (2018) Evaluating epistemic commitments and science communication practice in transdisciplinary research. Sci Commun 40(4):499–523. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547018786566

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Supran G, Oreskes N (2017) Assessing ExxonMobil’s climate change communications (1977–2014). Environ Res Lett 12(8):084019. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aa815f

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Tesler M (2018) Elite domination of public doubts about climate change (not evolution). Polit Commun 35(2):306–326. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2017.1380092

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. United States Census. (2019). U.S. Census Bureau QuickFacts: Oregon. https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/OR. Accessed 28 Mar 2020

  62. van der Sanden MCA, Meijman FJ (2008) Dialogue guides awareness and understanding of science: an essay on different goals of dialogue leading to different science communication approaches. Public Underst Sci 17(1):89–103. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662506067376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Whitmarsh D, Palmieri MG (2009) Social acceptability of marine aquaculture: the use of survey-based methods for eliciting public and stakeholder preferences. Mar Policy 33(3):452–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.marpol.2008.10.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Whitmarsh L (2011) Scepticism and uncertainty about climate change: dimensions, determinants and change over time. Glob Environ Chang 21(2):690–700. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2011.01.016

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Yeo SK, Xenos MA, Brossard D, Scheufele DA (2015) Selecting our own science: how communication contexts and individual traits shape information seeking. Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci 658(1):172–191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716214557782

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The research leading to these results received funding from the Faculty Development Program at Portland State University.

Availability of data and materials

Data are not available.

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors both made substantial contributions to this study. BS led research design and manuscript preparation. DTR led data analysis and aided in manuscript preparation.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brianne Suldovsky.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

This study was reviewed and approved by the Portland State University Institutional Review Board (Office of Research Integrity), Reference no. 196349-18.

Consent to participate

Participants were provided with an informed consent form prior to participation in this survey that was reviewed and approved by the Portland State University Institutional Review Board.

Consent for publication

All authors read and approved the final manuscript and consent to publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

ESM 1

(DOCX 16.1 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Suldovsky, B., Taylor-Rodríguez, D. Epistemic engagement: examining personal epistemology and engagement preferences with climate change in Oregon. Climatic Change 166, 48 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-021-03138-5

Download citation

Keywords

  • Public engagement
  • Climate change
  • Personal epistemology