Climatic Change

, Volume 141, Issue 2, pp 167–177 | Cite as

Brief exposure to Pope Francis heightens moral beliefs about climate change

  • Jonathon P. Schuldt
  • Adam R. Pearson
  • Rainer Romero-Canyas
  • Dylan Larson-Konar
Article

Abstract

In his recent encyclical letter Laudato Si’: On Care for Our Common Home, Pope Francis issued a moral appeal to the global community for swift action on climate change. However, social science research suggests a complex relationship between religious concepts and environmental attitudes, raising the question of what influence the pope’s position may have on public opinion regarding this polarizing issue. In a national probability survey experiment of U.S. adults (n = 1212), we find that brief exposure to Pope Francis influenced the climate-related beliefs of broad segments of the public: it increased perceptions of climate change as a moral issue for the overall sample (and among Republicans in particular) and increased felt personal responsibility for contributing to climate change and its mitigation (among Democrats). Moreover, prior awareness of the pope’s views on climate change mattered, such that those who indicated greater awareness of the pope’s position showed stronger treatment effects, consistent with a priming account of these effects. Results complement recent correlational findings and offer further evidence of the Vatican’s influence on climate change public opinion.

References

  1. Bargh JA, Morsella E (2008) The unconscious mind. Perspect Psychol Sci 3(1):73–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Burke M, Hsiang SM, Miguel E (2015) Global non-linear effect of temperature on economic production. Nature 527:235–239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Clements JM, McCright AM, Xiao C (2014) Green Christians? An empirical examination of environmental concern within the US general public. Organ Environ 27(1):85–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Feinberg M, Willer R (2013) The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychol Sci 24(1):56–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Fishbein M, Yzer MC (2003) Using theory to design effective health behavior interventions. Commun Theory 13(2):164–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Francis P (2015) On Care for our common home. Vatican City, Italy: Encyclical Letter, Libreria Editrice Vaticana. [Accessed 07-08-2016] Available online at http://w2.vatican.va/content/francesco/en/encyclicals/documents/papafrancesco_20150524_enciclicalaudato-si.html
  7. Graham J, Meindl P, Beall E, Johnson KM, Zhang L (2016) Cultural differences in moral judgment and behavior, across and within societies. Curr Opin Psychol 8:125–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Haidt J (2007) The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science 316(5827):998–1002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hamilton LC, Hartter J, Lemcke-Stampone M, Moore DW, Safford TG (2015) Tracking public beliefs about anthropogenic climate change. PLoS ONE 10(9):e0138208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Higgins ET (1989) Knowledge accessibility and activation: subjectivity and suffering from unconscious sources. Unintended Thought 3:75–123Google Scholar
  11. Hornik R, Woolf KD (1999) Using cross-sectional surveys to plan message strategies. Soc Mark Q 5(2):34–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Jones RP, Cox D & Navarro-Rivera J (2014) Believers, sympathizers, and skeptics: why Americans are conflicted about climate change, environmental policy and science: findings from the PRRI/AAR religions, values, and climate change survey. Public Religion Research Institute and American Academy of Religion. Released November 22, 2014Google Scholar
  13. Leiserowitz A, & Akerlof K (2010) Race, ethnicity and public responses to climate change. Yale University and George Mason University. New Haven, CT: Yale Program on Climate Change CommunicationGoogle Scholar
  14. Li N, Hilgard J, Scheufele DA, Winneg KM, Jamieson KH (2016) Cross-pressuring conservative Catholics? Effects of Pope Francis’ encyclical on the US public opinion on climate change. Clim Chang 139:367–380Google Scholar
  15. Loersch C, Payne BK (2011) The situated inference model: an integrative account of the effects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation. Perspect Psychol Sci 6(3):234–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Maibach E, Leiserowitz A, Roser-Renouf C, Myers T, Rosenthal S & Feinberg G (2015) The Francis effect: how Pope Francis changed the conversation about global warming. George Mason University and Yale University. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University Center for Climate Change CommunicationGoogle Scholar
  17. Markowitz EM (2012) Is climate change an ethical issue? Examining young adults’ beliefs about climate and morality. Clim Chang 114(3–4):479–495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McCright AM, Dunlap RE (2011) The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. Sociol Q 52(2):155–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nisbet MC, Markowitz EM & Kotcher JE (2012) Winning the conversation: framing and moral messaging in environmental campaigns. Talking Green: Exploring Contemporary Issues in Environmental Communications, 9–36Google Scholar
  20. O’Neill E (2016) The Pope and the environment: towards an integral ecology? Environ Polit 25(4):749–754CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Pew (2014) Pope Francis’ image positive in much of the world. Available online at http://www.pewglobal.org/2014/12/11/pope-francis-image-positive-in-much-of-world/
  22. Roser-Renouf C, Maibach E, Leiserowitz A & Rosenthal S (2016) Global warming, God, and the “end times”: March, 2016. Yale University and George Mason University. New Haven, CT: Yale Program on Climate Change CommunicationGoogle Scholar
  23. Rottman J, Kelemen D, Young L (2015) Hindering harm and preserving purity: how can moral psychology save the planet? Philos Compass 10(2):134–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Schuldt JP, Pearson AR (2016) The role of race and ethnicity in climate change polarization: evidence from a U.S. national survey experiment. Clim Chang 136:495–505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Schuman H & Presser S (1996) Questions and answers in attitude surveys: experiments on question form, wording, and context. SageGoogle Scholar
  26. Schwarz N (1998) Accessible content and accessibility experiences: the interplay of declarative and experiential information in judgment. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 2(2):87–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Schwarz N (1999) Self-reports: how the questions shape the answers. Am Psychol 54(2):93–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Srull TK, Wyer RS (1979) The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: some determinants and implications. J Pers Soc Psychol 37(10):1660–1672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Stern PC, Dietz T, Black JS (1985) Support for environmental protection: the role of moral norms. Popul Environ 8(3–4):204–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Stern PC, Dietz T, Abel TD, Guagnano GA, Kalof L (1999) A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: the case of environmentalism. Hum Ecol Rev 6(2):81–97Google Scholar
  31. Suhay E, Druckman JN (2015) The politics of science political values and the production, communication, and reception of scientific knowledge. Ann Am Acade Polit Soc Sci 658(1):6–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Tourangeau R, Rips LJ & Rasinski K (2000) The psychology of survey response. Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  33. Van Zant AB, Moore DA (2015) Leaders’ use of moral justifications increases policy support. Psychol Sci 26(6):934–943CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wardekker JA, Petersen AC, van Der Sluijs JP (2009) Ethics and public perception of climate change: exploring the Christian voices in the US public debate. Glob Environ Chang 19(4):512–521CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Wolsko C, Ariceaga H, Seiden J (2016) Red, white, and blue enough to be green: effects of moral framing on climate change attitudes and conservation behaviors. J Exp Soc Psychol 65:7–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jonathon P. Schuldt
    • 1
  • Adam R. Pearson
    • 2
  • Rainer Romero-Canyas
    • 3
  • Dylan Larson-Konar
    • 4
  1. 1.Cornell UniversityIthacaUSA
  2. 2.Pomona CollegeClaremontUSA
  3. 3.Environmental Defense Fund and Columbia UniversityNew YorkUSA
  4. 4.Environmental Defense FundNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations