Abstract
Background
The association between problem behavior in children and adolescents and creativity has hardly been examined. The few studies available report contradictory findings. There is some empirical evidence that a low socioeconomic status coincides with lower creativity test achievements.
Objective
The current study addresses the question whether problem behavior is relevant to explain variance in creativity test achievements while controlling for socioeconomic status differences.
Method
The sample comprises N = 234 children and adolescents from Germany (age range 10–14, 115 female) including 193 above cut-off in a problem behavior screening (SDQ). Creativity is operationalized by a standardized nonverbal drawing test (TCT-DP). Hierarchic regression analyses are used to analyze the impact of problem behavior on creativity.
Results
Problem behavior explains considerable variance in creativity test performance. Whereas internalizing problems are negatively associated with creativity, there is a moderate positive correlation between dissocial problem behavior and creativity. Hyperactive-inattentive children and adolescents achieve better creativity test results than controls. Socioeconomic status and gender both have a moderating impact, but do not level out the link between problem behavior and creativity.
Conclusions
The results underline the potential importance of nonverbal creativity as a behavioral, cognitive and emotional feature of inattentive and hyperactive children and adolescents, which could be beneficial in school and other intervention contexts.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Abraham, A., Windmann, S., Siefen, R., Daum, I., & Güntürkün, O. (2006). Creative thinking in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology,12(2), 111–123.
Averill, J. R. (1999). Individual differences in emotional creativity: Structure and correlates. Journal of Personality,67(2), 331–371.
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science,23(10), 1117–1122.
Barbot, B. (2016). Perspectives on creativity development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Beaujean, A. A., Parker, S., & Qiu, X. (2013). The relationship between cognitive ability and depression: A longitudinal data analysis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,48(12), 1983–1992.
Boot, N., Nevicka, B., & Baas, M. (2017). Subclinical symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are associated with specific creative processes. Personality and Individual Differences,114, 73–81.
Brandau, H., Daghofer, F., Hollerer, L., Kaschnitz, W., Kellner, K., Kirchmair, G., et al. (2007). The relationship between creativity, teacher ratings on behavior, age, and gender in pupils from seven to ten years. The Journal of Creative Behavior,41(2), 91–113.
Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung [Federal Agency for Civic Education]. (2019). Daten-report 2016. Ein Sozialbericht für die Bundesrepublik Deutschland [Data Report 2016. A social report for the Federal Republic of Germany]. https://www.destatis.de/DE/Publikationen/Datenreport/Downloads/Datenreport2016.pdf?__blob=publicationFile. Accessed 18 December 2019.
Byron, K., & Khazanchi, S. (2011). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship of state and trait anxiety to performance on figural and verbal creative tasks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,37(2), 269–283.
Carlsson, I., Wendt, P. E., & Risberg, J. (2000). On the neurobiology of creativity. Differences in frontal activity between high and low creative subjects. Neuropsychologia,38(6), 873–885.
Castillo-Vergara, M., Barrios Galleguillos, N., Cuello, L. F., Alvarez-Marin, A., & Acuña-Opazo, C. (2018). Does socioeconomic status influence student creativity? Thinking Skills and Creativity,29, 142–152.
Chang, J., Chen, H., Hsu, C., Chan, Y., & Chang, Y. (2015). Flexible humor styles and the creative mind: Using a typological approach to investigate the relationship between humor styles and creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts,9(3), 306–312.
Costello, C. G. (2016). Depression: Loss of reinforcers or loss of reinforcer effectiveness? Republished article. Behavior Therapy,47(5), 595–599.
Dai, D. Y., Tan, X., Marathe, D., Valtcheva, A., Pruzek, R. M., & Shen, J. (2012). Influences of social and educational environments on creativity during adolescence: Does SES matter? Creativity Research Journal,24(2–3), 191–199.
Drake, J. E. (2019). Examining the psychological and psychophysiological benefits of drawing over one month. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,13(3), 338–347.
Essau, C. A., Olaya, B., Anastassiou‐Hadjicharalambous, X., Pauli, G., Gilvarry, C., Bray, D., et al. (2012). Psychometric properties of the strength and difficulties questionnaire from five European countries. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(3), 232–245.
Forgeard, M. J. (2011). Happy people thrive on adversity: Pre-existing mood moderates the effect of emotion inductions on creative thinking. Personality and Individual Differences,51(8), 904–909.
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric Properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,40(11), 1337–1345.
Guilford, J. P. (1956). The structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin,53(4), 267–293.
Harris, D. J., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2015). Fast and furious: The influence of implicit aggression, premeditation, and provoking situations on malevolent creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,9(1), 54–64.
He, M. W.-J., Wong, W.-C., & Hui, A. N.-N. (2015). Gender differences in means and variability on creative thinking: Patterns in childhood, adolescence, and emerging adulthood. In A.-G. Tan & C. Perleth (Eds.), Creativity, culture, and development (pp. 85–98). New York, NY: Springer.
Healey, D. (2014). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and creativity: Ever the twain shall meet? In J. C. Kaufman & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Creativity and mental illness (pp. 236–249). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson, S., Hollis, C., Marlow, N., Simms, V., & Wolke, D. (2014). Screening for childhood mental health disorders using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: The validity of multi-informant reports. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,56, 453–459.
Kaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2016). Creativity and reason in cognitive development (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Klasen, F., Petermann, F., Meyrose, A., Barkmann, C., Otto, C., Haller, A., et al. (2016). Verlauf psychischer Auffälligkeiten von Kindern und Jugendlichen. Ergebnisse der BELLA-Kohortenstudie. Kindheit und Entwicklung,25(1), 10–20.
Kroesbergen, E. H., van Hooijdonk, M., Van Viersen, S., Middel-Lalleman, M. N., & Reijnders, J. W. (2016). The psychological well-being of early identified gifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly,60(1), 16–30.
Kusá, D. (1999). Creativity and conformity: Quality and polarity between the two. Studia Psychologica,41(4), 337–339.
Lau, S., & Cheung, P. C. (2010). Developmental trends of creativity: What twists of turn do boys and girls take at different grades? Creativity Research Journal,22(3), 329–336.
Linderkamp, F. & Grünke, M. (2007). Lern- und Verhaltensstörungen: Klassifikation, Prävalenz & Prognostik. [Learning and Behavior Problems: Classification, Prevalence & Prognostics]. In F. Linderkamp & M. Grünke (Hrsg.), Lern- und Verhaltensstörungen. Genese, Diagnostik & Intervention [Learning and Behavior Problems. Development, Diagnostics & Intervention]. (S. 14–28). Weinheim: Beltz.
Lüdeke, S. (2018). Verhaltensprobleme bei Jugendlichen. Zur Stressverarbeitung in freundschaftlichen und romantischen Peerbeziehungen. [Behavior problems in adolescents. An empirical study about stress processing in friendship and romantic peer relations]. Wiesbaden: Springer.
Lüdeke, S., & Linderkamp, F. (2018). Beurteilerdiskrepanzen als Indikatoren für Schulstress. Eine Studie zu Einschätzungen internalisierender und externalisierender Verhaltensprobleme aus Sicht von Jugendlichen und Lehrpersonen. [Informant discrepancies as indicators of school stress. A study about the assessment of internalizing and externalizing problem behavior rated by adolescents and their teachers]. Empirische Sonderpädagogik,4, 353–369.
Lüdeke, S., Linderkamp, F., & Cevani, I. (2019). Differenzielle Analysen der Kreativität bei Kindern und Jugendlichen mit ADHS. [Differential analyses of the relationship between creativity and ADHD in children and adolescents]. Kindheit und Entwicklung,28(2), 106–113.
Lüdeke, S., Linderkamp, F., Weidenfeld, A., & Borusiak, P. (2015). Die Einflüsse des Migrationshintergrundes und psychischer Störungen auf kognitive Leistungsdaten einer SPZ-Inanspruchnahmepopulation. [The Impact of Migration Background and Psychiatric Disorders on Cognitive Achievement of a Pediatric Outpatient Sample]. Kindheit und Entwicklung,24(4), 236–242.
Ogletree, E. (1971). A cross-cultural examination of the creative thinking ability of public and private school pupils in England, Scotland, and Germany. The Journal of Social Psychology,83(2), 301–302.
Paek, S. H., Abdulla, A. M., & Cramond, B. (2016). A meta-analysis of the relationship between three common psychopathologies-ADHD, anxiety, and depression- and indicators of little-c creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly,60(2), 117–133.
Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologists? Potentials, pitfalls, and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist,39, 83–96.
Roy, B. V., Veenstra, M., & Clench-Aas, J. (2008). Construct validity of the five-factor Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in pre-, early, and late adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,49(12), 1304–1312.
Shaw, G. A. (1992). Hyperactivity and creativity: The tacit dimension. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,30(2), 157–160.
Stoltzfus, G., Nibbelink, B. L., Vredenburg, D., & Thyrum, E. (2011). Gender, gender role, and creativity. Social Behavior and Personality,39(3), 425–432.
Tacher, E. L., & Readdick, C. A. (2006). The relation between aggression and creativity among second graders. Creativity Research Journal,18(3), 261–267.
Teigen, K. H. (1994). Yerkes-Dodson: A law for all seasons. Theory and Psychology,4(4), 525–547.
Tsao, Y. (2008). Using guided play to enhance children’s conversation, creativity and competence in literacy. Education,128, 515–520.
Urban, K. K. (2011). Möglichkeiten und Grenzen von Kreativitätsdiagnostik. [Possibilities and Limitations of Diagnosing Creativity]. In C. Koop (Hrsg.), Kreativität: Zufall oder harte Arbeit? [Creativity - does it just happen by chance or is it hard work] (pp. 18–27). Frankfurt am Main: Karg Stiftung.
Urban, K. K., & Jellen, H. G. (2010). TSD-Z. Test zum Schöpferischen Denken - Zeichnerisch. [TCT-DP. Test for creative thinking drawing production]. Frankfurt am Main: Pearson Assessment & Information GmbH.
van den Akker, A. L., Prinzie, P., & Overbeek, G. (2016). Dimensions of personality pathology in adolescence: Longitudinal associations with big five personality dimensions across childhood and adolescence. Journal of Personality Disorders,30(2), 211–231.
Verhaeghen, P., Joorman, J., & Khan, R. (2005). Why we sing the blues: The relation between self-reflective rumination, mood, and creativity. Emotion,5(2), 226.
Wangu, R. S., & Manral, J. B. (1990). Intelligence, creativity and indisciplined behaviour of university students. Indian Journal of Psychometry and Education,21(2), 57–61.
Williams, D. R., Priest, N., & Anderson, N. B. (2016). Understanding associations among race, socioeconomic status, and health: Patterns and prospects. Health Psychology,35(4), 407–411.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical Approval
All procedures performed in our study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The treatment of our study’s human participants also complies with APA ethical standards.
Informed Consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Lüdeke, S., Linderkamp, F., Baumann, T. et al. Empirical Analysis of Creativity in Children and Adolescents with Internalizing and Externalizing Problem Behavior. Child Youth Care Forum 49, 603–621 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-020-09546-5
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-020-09546-5
Keywords
- Creativity
- Internalizing
- Externalizing
- Problem behavior
- Children and adolescents