Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

To Punish or Not to Punish? The Impact of Tax Fraud Punishment on Observers’ Tax Compliance

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article synthesizes insights from deterrence theory and social psychology literature on retributive justice to develop and test a theoretical model which predicts how and why observers’ tax compliance intentions are influenced by knowledge of the punitive outcomes faced by individuals found guilty of tax fraud. We test our model experimentally on a sample of Canadian taxpayers and manipulate perceived responsibility for a fraud and whether a fraud perpetrator is punished. We show that observers’ tax compliance increases when a fraud perpetrator is punished only when the perpetrator is perceived as blameworthy. The psychological process through which this positive influence operates is relatively complex, as it includes perceptions of punishment deservingness and affect. We also find that tax compliance decreases when a tax fraud perpetrator is unpunished, regardless of perceived blameworthiness. Our results are robust when we control for economic determinants known to influence fraud. The article concludes by discussing our findings’ implications for fraud research and policy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Explore related subjects

Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.

Data Availability

Experimental data is available upon request.

Notes

  1. One of these is the KPMG-Isle of Man tax scheme in Canada, which we adapt in our experimental setting.

  2. Anand et al. (2015) similarly observe that much research examines fraud occurring within an organization, focusing on fraud perpetration and prevention.

  3. In Feather’s (1998) model, seriousness of offense is endogenous to responsibility and harshness of penalty is endogenous to punishment deservingness. In our study, we control for these variables.

  4. We examine affect as a positive (pleasure) or negative (displeasure) response related to an event. This is consistent with similar research examining the role of affect in judgment and decision making (e.g., Mercer, 2005).

  5. The Investigative Consortium of Investigate Journalists documents that some, but not all, tax authorities punished parties identified in the Panama Papers (Wilson-Chapman et al., 2019).

  6. We do not expect the presence or absence of a retributive outcome to moderate the association between responsibility and deservingness. Feather (1996, p. 273) notes that deservingness affects judgments about a penalty, which implies that a penalty (punishment) occurs after deservingness perceptions are formed. As shown in our supplemental analysis, there is no significant interaction effect of punishment occurrence and responsibility on deservingness.

  7. Tax compliance intentions are a proxy for tax behavior. As Bobek et al. (2013, p. 458) state, “Concerns that behavioral intentions might not map directly to taxpayers’ actual behavior in a situation are partially mitigated by the fact that a joint investigation of actual and hypothetical tax evaders (Webley et al., 2001) indicated that both groups had similar attitudes and motivations”. As well, there is strong empirical support (Sheeran, 2002) for several psychological models, including the theory of planned behavior (Carpenter & Reimers, 2005), that affirm that an individual’s intention is the strongest predictor of their behavior. In the tax compliance literature, it is common to measure tax compliance intentions rather than actual tax behavior due to social desirability bias (e.g., Blanthorne & Kaplan, 2008; Bobek et al., 2013; Farrar et al., 2019; Verboon & Van Dijke, 2007).

  8. These aforementioned streams of literature do not enable us to predict direct effects of responsibility, punishment occurrence, or punishment deservingness on tax compliance intentions, nor do they allow us to predict a direct effect of responsibility on affective reactions. Nevertheless, we investigate non-hypothesized effects in a subsequent supplemental analysis.

  9. Sample sizes per cell varied from 97 to 102. Our Qualtrics software indicated that the quotas had been reached when the total participants were at 399 rather than 400.

  10. Our sample appears reasonably representative of the Canadian population with respect to age and income. According to Statistics Canada (2021a, 2021b), the median age for all Canadians in 2020 was 40.9. The median age of our sample is 32.0, but includes individuals ages 20–70. In 2019, the median income in Canada by age group was $41,700 (ages 25–34), $51,500 (ages 35–44), $54,200 (ages 45–54), $42,800 (ages 55–64), and $30,400 (age 65 and over) (Statistics Canada 2021a, 2021b). When we compared our data segmented similarly by age group, the median score for income category was 3 ($50,000–$74,999) for all age categories except age 65 and over, which had a median score of 2 ($25,000–$49,999).

  11. In our experimental design, we decided not to manipulate the magnitude of the dollars evaded. This design choice was made as we are more interested in whether or not a punishment occurs. Furthermore, not manipulating the magnitude of dollars allows for a cleaner experimental design that reduces a possible confound between magnitude of dollars and punishment.

  12. Except for the four tax compliance statements and attention-check and manipulation check questions, all other questionnaire items prior to the demographic measures were presented in random order across several screens. Across the entire instrument, for any measures with multiple items, the groups of items were presented in random order.

  13. Responsibility is manipulated as an ex-ante construct, as opposed to ex-post. Particularly, offenders are perceived as being responsible for the tax fraud ex-ante, whether or not the offender accepts this responsibility ex-post. In the statistical analysis, the higher responsibility condition is coded as ‘ + 1’, whereas the lower responsibility condition is coded as ‘0’.

  14. A news story (Cashore, 2015) reports that one of KPMGs clients who invested in the Isle of Man tax scheme said that he was unaware of Canadian tax laws when he emigrated from South Africa in the mid-1990s. The taxpayer said, “I went to the best people in the country. I’m being drawn into this, and I don’t think I should have been in the first place.” It is unclear whether an observer would perceive that a client in this type of situation is responsible for the tax fraud, or not.

  15. In subsection 239(1) of the Income Tax Act, R.S.C. 1985 c. 1 (5th Supp.), and subsection 380(1) of the Criminal Code of Canada, R.S.C. 1985 c. C-46.

  16. In the statistical analysis, the condition where punishment occurred is coded as ‘ + 1’, whereas the condition where punishment did not occur is coded as ‘0’.

  17. This manipulation intentionally does not make clear whether the tax authority chose not to prosecute the tax evaders, or chose to prosecute the tax evaders but was unsuccessful in the prosecution attempt, as doing so could limit the generalizability of our findings. Thus, the manipulation allows for both possibilities. In the Isle of Man tax dodge, the Canadian tax authority chose not to prosecute the tax evaders. We discuss this issue further in the final section of the manuscript.

  18. In Feather (1996, 1998), participants rated the extent to which punishment was deserving using a 7-point scale with endpoints of ‘doesn’t deserve it at all’ (1) and ‘deserves it a lot’ (7).

  19. The tax compliance statements were solicited from a third-person perspective, which is common in tax compliance research, as it minimizes participant discomfort and reduces the risk that social desirability bias would contaminate the results (Armacost et al., 1991). Similarly, respondents were told to imagine that they were Chris when responding to all other questions.

  20. Given that the Isle of Man tax dodge was publicized in 2017, and participants completed this survey in 2020, it is reasonable that few participants were aware of it. As well, given the robustness of our subsequent results, it is likely that the results would be even stronger if the research were conducted while a media story about a tax dodge was publicized.

  21. As reported in Panel B of Table 3, the index of moderated mediation when all ten covariates are present is 0.049 and is significant, as a bootstrap confidence interval is entirely above zero (0.002–0.1283). The index of moderated mediation when no covariates are present is 0.101, and is also significant, as a bootstrap confidence interval is entirely above zero (0.013–0.239).

  22. If participants answered the attention check question incorrectly, they are prompted that the response is incorrect, and are provided with a hint. Thus, participants have another opportunity to answer the attention check question and continue with the experiment (c.f. Berger et al., 2020). Among participants, 2.3% (or 9) of them fail this attention check question once. As analyses yield identical statistical inferences if these responses are excluded, we include all responses in our analyses.

  23. In an ANCOVA with the ten significant covariates, the main effect of punishment occurrence on tax compliance intentions remains significant (F = 4.06, p = .045). The adjusted R-squared of the ANOVA (ANCOVA) is 0.025 (0.319).

  24. Our model corresponds to Hayes’ Model 91 except that we also have a direct path from the moderator to the dependent variable. We use cmatrix syntax to test our model (Hayes, 2018a, p. 653).

  25. This model corresponds to Hayes (2018a) Model 92.

  26. Government of Canada, Finance Committee meeting May 5, 2016. Online: https://openparliament.ca/committees/finance/42-1/18/.

References

  • Ainley, M. (2006). Connecting with learning: Motivation, affect and cognition in interest processes. Educational Psychology Review, 18(4), 391–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allingham, M., & Sandmo, A. (1972). Income tax evasion: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Public Economics, 1(3–4), 323–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J. (2012). Measuring, explaining and controlling tax evasion: Lessons from theory, experiments, and field studies. International Tax and Public Finance, 19(1), 54–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J. (2019). What motivates tax compliance? Journal of Economic Surveys, 33(2), 353–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Kirchler, E., & Muelbacher, S. (2012). Combining psychology and economics in the analysis of compliance: From enforcement to cooperation. Economic Analysis and Policy, 42(2), 133–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anand, V., Dacin, M. T., & Murphy, P. (2015). The continued need for diversity in fraud research. Journal of Business Ethics, 131(4), 751–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., Erard, B., & Feinstein, J. (1998). Tax compliance. Journal of Economic Literature, 36(2), 818–860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armacost, R., Hosseini, J., Morris, S., & Rehbein, K. (1991). An empirical comparison of direct questioning, scenario, and randomized response methods for obtaining sensitive business information. Decision Sciences, 22(5), 1073–1090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: An economic approach. Journal of Political Economy, 78(2), 169–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, L., Guo, L., & King, T. (2020). Selfish sharing? The impact of the sharing economy on tax reporting honesty. Journal of Business Ethics, 167(2), 181–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, L., Perreault, S., & Wainberg, J. (2017). Hijacking the moral imperative: How financial incentives can discourage whistleblower reporting. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 36(3), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blank, J., & Levin, D. (2010). When is tax enforcement publicized? Virginia Tax Review, 30(1), 1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanthorne, C., & Kaplan, S. (2008). An egocentric model of the relations among the opportunity to underreport, social norms, ethical beliefs, and underreporting behavior. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 33(7–8), 684–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D., Hageman, A., & Kelliher, C. (2013). Analyzing the role of social norms in tax compliance behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 115(3), 451–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D., Roberts, R., & Sweeney, J. (2007). The social norms of tax compliance: Evidence from Australia, Singapore, and the United States. Journal of Business Ethics, 74(1), 49–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braithwaite, V., & Braithwaite, J. (2000). An evolving model for tax enforcement. In N. Shover & J. Wright (Eds.), Crimes of privilege: Readings in white-collar crime (pp. 1–19). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. (2014). Advantageous comparison and rationalization of earnings management. Journal of Accounting Research, 52(4), 849–876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T., & Reimers, J. (2005). Unethical and fraudulent financial reporting: Applying the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 60(2), 115–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cashore, H. (2015). KPMG offshore ‘sham’ deceived tax authorities, CRA alleges. https://www.cbc.ca/news/business/kpmg-offshore-sham-deceived-tax-authorities-cra-alleges-1.3209838

  • Christian, R., & Alm, J. (2014). Empathy, sympathy, and tax compliance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 40(1), 62–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collum, N., Jurney, S., & Marshall, M. (2021). Investigating the effect of service messages on noncompliant taxpayers’ reactions to declining audit effectiveness. Working paper.

  • Colquitt, J. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 386–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darley, J., & Pittman, T. (2003). The psychology of compensatory and retributive justice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(3), 324–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Datt, K. (2016). To shame or not to shame: That is the question. eJournal of Tax Research, 14(2), 486–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, S., & Johnstone, K. (2018). A natural field experiment examining the joint role of audit partner leadership and subordinates’ knowledge in fraud brainstorming. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 66, 14–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devos, K., & Zackrisson, M. (2015). Tax compliance and the public disclosure of tax information: An Australia/Norway comparison. eJournal of Tax Research, 13(1), 108–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrar, J., Kaplan, S., & Thorne, L. (2019). The effect of interactional fairness and detection expectations on taxpayers’ compliance intentions. Journal of Business Ethics, 154(1), 167–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N. (1996). Reactions to penalties for an offense in relation to authoritarianism, values, perceived responsibility, perceived seriousness, and deservingness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3), 571–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N. (1998). Reactions to penalties for offenses committed by the police and public citizens: Testing a social-cognitive process model of retributive justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 528–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N. (1999). Judgments of deservingness: Studies in the psychology of justice and achievement. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(2), 86–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N., & Sherman, R. (2002). Envy, resentment, schadenfreude, and sympathy: Reactions to deserved and undeserved achievement and subsequent failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(7), 953–961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N., Wenzel, M., & McKee, I. (2013). Integrating multiple perspectives on schadenfreude: The role of deservingness and emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 37(3), 574–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feld, L., & Frey, B. (2007). Tax compliance as the result of a psychological tax contract: The role of incentives and responsive regulation. Law & Policy, 29(1), 102–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Free, C., & Murphy, P. (2015). The ties that bind: The decision to co-offend in fraud. Contemporary Accounting Research, 32(1), 18–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., Weiner, B., & Zucker, G. (1997). An attributional analysis of punishment goals and public reactions to O. J. Simpson. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(4), 331–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. (2018a). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. (2018b). Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation: Quantification, inference, and interpretation. Communication Monographs, 85(1), 4–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structural analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, R., & Huby, M. (2004). The construction and interpretation of vignettes in social research. Social Work and Social Sciences Review, 11(1), 36–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, C., Evans, J., & Moser, D. (2005). Economic and equity effects on tax reporting decisions. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 30(7/8), 609–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E. (2007). The economic psychology of tax behaviour. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchsteiger, G., Rigotti, L., & Rustichini, A. (2006). Your morals might be your moods. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 59(2), 155–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kogler, C., Muehlbacher, S., & Kirchler, E. (2015). Testing the “slippery slope framework” among self-employed taxpayers. Economics of Governance, 16(2), 125–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornhauser, M. (2007). Tax morale approach to compliance: Recommendations for the IRS. Florida Tax Review, 8(6), 599–640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lederman, L. (2003). The interplay between norms and enforcement in tax compliance. Ohio State Law Journal, 64(6), 1453–1514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maciejovsky, B., Schwarzenberger, H., & Kirchler, E. (2012). Rationality versus emotions: The case of tax ethics and compliance. Journal of Business Ethics, 109(3), 339–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahangila, D., & Holland, K. (2015). Analysis of procedural and retributive justice in tax compliance. The Operations Research Society of Eastern Africa, 5(1), 113–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayhew, B., & Murphy, P. (2014). The impact of authority on reporting behavior, rationalization and affect. Contemporary Accounting Research, 31(2), 420–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazza, S. (2003). Taxpayer privacy and tax compliance. University of Kansas Law Review, 51(5), 1065–1144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, M. (2005). The fleeting effects of disclosure forthcomingness on management’s reporting credibility. The Accounting Review, 80(2), 723–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D., & Vidmar, N. (1981). The social psychology of punishment reactions. In M. Lerner & S. Lerner (Eds.), The justice motive in social behavior (pp. 145–172). Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Molero, J., & Pujol, F. (2012). Walking inside the potential tax evader’s mind: Tax morale does matter. Journal of Business Ethics, 105(2), 151–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, P., & Dacin, M. (2011). Psychological pathways to fraud: Understanding and preventing fraud in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 101(4), 601–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2019). Combatting tax crimes more effectively in APEC economies. Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development and Asia-Pacific Economic Cooperation. http://www.oecd.org/tax/crime/combatting-tax-crimes-more-effectively-in-apec-economies.htm

  • Peer, E., Brandimarte, L., Samat, S., & Acquisti, A. (2017). Beyond the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 153–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelletier, K., & Bligh, M. (2008). The aftermath of organizational corruption: Employee attributions and emotional reactions. Journal of Business Ethics, 80(4), 823–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piquero, A., Paternoster, R., Pogarsky, G., & Loughran, T. (2011). Elaborating the individual difference component in deterrence theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 7, 335–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, T. C., Cullen, F. T., Blevins, K. R., Daigle, L. E., & Madensen, T. D. (2006). The empirical status of deterrence theory: A meta-analysis. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. R. Blevins (Eds.), Advances in criminological theory: Vol. 15. Taking stock: The status of criminological theory (p. 367–395). Transaction Publishers.

  • Privitera, A., Enachescu, J., Kirchler, E., & Hartmann, A. J. (2021). Emotions in tax-related situations shape compliance intentions: A comparison between Austria and Italy. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 92, 101698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Randall, D. (1989). Taking stock: Can the theory of reasoned action explain unethical conduct? Journal of Business Ethics, 8(11), 873–882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raskolnikov, A. (2009). Revealing choices: Using taxpayer choice to target tax enforcement. Columbia Law Review, 109(4), 689–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear and appeals in attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In B. Cacioppo & L. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A source book (pp. 153–176). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, T., Schleifer, M., & Altman, I. (1981). Judgments of causation, responsibility, and punishment in cases of harm-doing. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Sciences, 13(3), 238–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. European Review of Social Psychology, 12(1), 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skarlicki, D., & Folger, R. (1997). Retaliation in the workplace: The roles of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(3), 434–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slemrod, J. (2019). Tax compliance and enforcement. Journal of Economic Literature, 57(4), 904–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A., & Reece-Greenhalgh, D. (2017). Don’t expect prosecutions to come from the Paradise Papers. https://www.lexology.com/library/detail.aspx?g=1a57b13e-698d-4aaf-a7ac-eb3e0452fb8c

  • Statistics Canada. (2021a). Table 17-10-0005-01 population estimates on July 1st, by age and sex. https://doi.org/10.25318/1710000501-eng

  • Statistics Canada. (2021b). Table 11-10-0239-01 income of individuals by age group, sex and income source, Canada, provinces and selected census metropolitan areas. https://doi.org/10.25318/1110023901-eng

  • Trivedi, V., Shehata, M., & Lynn, B. (2003). Impact of personal and situational factors on tax compliance: An experimental analysis. Journal of Business Ethics, 47(3), 175–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trompeter, G., Carpenter, T., Desai, N., Jones, K., & Riley, R. (2013). A synthesis of fraud-related research. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 32(Supplement), 287–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. (2012). Justice and effective cooperation. Social Justice Research, 25(4), 355–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K. (2003). On the subjective quality of social justice: The role of affect as information in the psychology of justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 482–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verboon, P., & Van Dijke, M. (2007). A self-interest analysis of justice and taxpayers’ compliance: How distributive justice moderates the effect of outcome favorability. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(6), 704–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verboon, P., & Van Dijke, M. (2011). When do severe sanctions enhance compliance? The role of procedural fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32(2), 120–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, J. (1992). Scenarios in business ethics research: Review, critical assessment, and recommendations. Business Ethics Quarterly, 2(2), 137–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webley, P., Cole, M., & Eidjar, O. (2001). The prediction of self-reported and hypothetical tax evasion: Evidence from England, France, and Norway. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22(2), 141–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, H., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18, 1–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2006). A letter from the tax office: Compliance effects of informational and interpersonal justice. Social Justice Research, 19(3), 345–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M., & Okimoto, T. (2016). Retributive justice. In C. Sabbagh & M. Schmitt (Eds.), Handbook of social justice theory and research (pp. 237–256). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M., Okimoto, T., Feather, N., & Platow, M. (2008). Retributive and restorative justice. Law and Human Behavior, 32(5), 375–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson-Chapman, A., Cucho, A., & Fitzgibbon, W. (2019). What happened after the Panama Papers? https://www.icij.org/investigations/panama-papers/what-happened-after-the-panama-papers/

  • Worsham, R. (1996). The effect of tax authority behavior on taxpayer compliance: A procedural justice approach. The Journal of the American Taxation Association, 18(2), 19–39.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank participants at the Canadian Academic Accounting Association annual meeting (2020), the Accounting, Behavior & Organizations research conference (2020), the Tax Administration Research Centre (Exeter, UK) annual conference (2020) and the East Coast Behavioural Accounting Workshop (Spring 2021) for helpful comments. We also thank Leslie Berger, Lan Guo, Rick Hatfield, Khim Kelly, Adam Presslee, Steven Salterio, Michael Wynes and workshop participants at the University of Waterloo and Wilfrid Laurier University for helpful comments. We gratefully acknowledge funding provided by the CPA Ontario Centre for Capital Markets and Behavioural Decision Making, Lazaridis School of Business and Economics, Wilfrid Laurier University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tisha King.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they do not have a conflict of interest.

Research Involving Human and/or Animal Participants

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the university’s research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix

Experimental Instrument

Part 1: Basic Tax Scenario, Common to All Experimental Materials

Chris is a local entrepreneur who owns a food truck. He receives a lot of cash from customers, which is difficult to keep track of. He is in the process of preparing this year’s tax return.

Recently, Chris read the following story in the news about some taxpayers.

Specific Scenario Information

No punishment, lower responsibility

CRA Uncovers Tax Scheme

Several years ago, a large accounting firm approached a number of wealthy clients. The accounting firm told them they could pay less taxes by investing funds in a company on the Isle of Man. The clients followed this advice, and made the investment.

Recently, the taxpayers were all audited by the Canada Revenue Agency (CRA). As it turned out, the tax shelter was a bogus arrangement that was intended to deceive the Canadian government. The taxpayers had to repay all the taxes they evaded, which is a standard practice and not a punishment.

However, the taxpayers were never punished. They were neither fined nor sentenced to jail.

Punishment, lower responsibility

CRA Uncovers Tax Scheme

Several years ago, a large accounting firm approached a number of wealthy clients. The accounting firm told them they could pay less taxes by investing funds in a company on the Isle of Man. The clients followed this advice, and made the investment.

Recently, the taxpayers were all audited by the Canada Revenue Agency (CRA). As it turned out, the tax shelter was a bogus arrangement that was intended to deceive the Canadian government. The taxpayers had to repay all the taxes they evaded, which is a standard practice and not a punishment.

Moreover, the taxpayers were then punished by being fined and sentenced to jail for several years.

Punishment, higher responsibility

CRA Uncovers Tax Scheme

Several years ago, a number of wealthy individuals read a brochure about paying less taxes by investing funds in a company on the Isle of Man. The individuals acted on their own, didn’t ask anyone for advice, and made the investment.

Recently, the taxpayers were all audited by the Canada Revenue Agency (CRA). As it turned out, the tax shelter was a bogus arrangement that was intended to deceive the Canadian government. The taxpayers had to repay all the taxes they evaded, which is a standard practice and not a punishment.

Moreover, the taxpayers were then punished by being fined and sentenced to jail for several years.

No punishment, higher responsibility

CRA Uncovers Tax Scheme

Several years ago, a number of wealthy individuals read a brochure about paying less taxes by investing funds in a company on the Isle of Man. The individuals acted on their own, didn’t ask anyone for advice, and made the investment.

Recently, the taxpayers were all audited by the Canada Revenue Agency (CRA). As it turned out, the tax shelter was a bogus arrangement that was intended to deceive the Canadian government. The taxpayers had to repay all the taxes they evaded, which is a standard practice and not a punishment.

However, the taxpayers were never punished. They were neither fined nor sentenced to jail.

Part 2: Questions

Thinking about the scenario you just read, please read the following statements and indicate your level of agreement by clicking on the appropriate response, where 1 = strongly disagree, and 7 = strongly agree.

Tax compliance intentions

  1. 1.

    Chris will not declare all the cash to the CRA.

  2. 2.

    Chris would be tempted to not report all of the cash receipts on the tax return.

  3. 3.

    Chris is unlikely to report all of the cash earnings to the CRA.

  4. 4.

    Under the circumstances, Chris might not report all of the cash earnings on the tax return.

Attention-check questions [note: #1 is also a manipulation check]

  1. 1.

    After the CRA’s audit, what happened to the taxpayers?

  2. (A)

    They had to repay the taxes they had evaded, and were fined and sentenced to jail.

  3. (B)

    They had to repay the taxes they had evaded, but were neither fined nor sentenced to jail.

  1. 2.

    In the scenario, what did the taxpayers decide to do?

  2. (A)

    Invest in a risky business venture in the Caribbean.

  3. (B)

    Invest in a company on the Isle of Man.

  4. (C)

    Provide financing to a mining company.

Responsibility manipulation effectiveness questions

  1. 1.

    The taxpayers have only themselves to blame for investing in the tax shelter scam. (1 = strongly agree; 7 = strongly disagree)

  2. 2.

    The taxpayers felt solely responsible for what happened with the tax shelter. (1 = strongly agree; 7 = strongly disagree)

Thinking about the scenario you just read, please read the following statements and indicate your level of agreement by clicking on the appropriate response, where 1 = strongly disagree, and 7 = strongly agree. Please answer these questions as if you were Chris.

Affective reactions

  1. 1.

    After the CRA’s audit, what happened to the taxpayers makes me angry. [reverse-scored]

  2. 2.

    After the CRA’s audit, what happened to the taxpayers makes me satisfied.

  3. 3.

    After the CRA’s audit, what happened to the taxpayers makes me disappointed. [reverse-scored]

  4. 4.

    After the CRA’s audit, what happened to the taxpayers makes me pleased.

Deservingness [generic]

  1. 1.

    The taxpayers who invested in the tax scheme deserved to be punished.

  2. 2.

    There should be negative consequences for the taxpayers who participated in the tax scheme.

  3. 3.

    The taxpayers who invested in the tax scheme need to be held accountable for their wrongdoing.

Severity of offense

  1. 1.

    I think tax evasion is a serious offense.

  2. 2.

    I think tax evasion is a severe crime.

Harshness of penalty

(1=much too lenient; 7=much too harsh)

  1. (1)

    What do you think of the end result to the taxpayers after the CRA’s investigation?

  2. (2)

    What do you think of the outcome the taxpayers received from the CRA?

  3. (3)

    What do you think of the final consequence to the taxpayers?

Justifying tax evasion

Can cheating on tax if you have the chance always be justified, never be justified, or something in between (1 = never justifiable; 10 = always justifiable).

Audit likelihood

Chris expects to be audited by the CRA.

Detection likelihood

If Chris does not report all the cash from customers on his tax return, he expects the CRA to find out.

Other Questions

Please provide the following demographic information about yourself.

  1. (1)

    What is your gender?    Male    Female     Other

  2. (2)

    In which year were you born?

  3. (3)

    How many years of work experience do you have? (round to nearest year)

  4. (4)

    Please indicate your highest level of education completed:

    Less than High School

    High School

    Associate’s Degree

    Bachelor’s Degree

    Master’s Degree

    Professional Degree or Doctoral Degree

    Other

  5. (5)

    Please indicate your approximate annual income (before-tax):

    Less than $25,000

    Between $25,000 and $49,999

    Between $50,000 and $74,999

    Between $75,000 and $99,999

    $100,000 or more

    Prefer not to answer

  6. (6)

    How would you categorize your political beliefs?

    Very conservative

    Moderately conservative

    Slightly conservative

    Middle of political spectrum

    Slightly liberal

    Moderately liberal

    Very liberal

Please answer the following final questions.

  1. (7)

    Before taking this survey, were you familiar with any news stories about tax schemes or tax havens? Yes  No.

  2. (8)

    Before taking this survey, were you aware of any Canadians linked to a secret tax dodge in the Isle of Man? Yes  No

  3. (9)

    Have you ever been audited by the CRA? Yes  No

  4. (10)

    Who usually prepares your tax return?

    I do

    My spouse/partner

    Paid preparer

    Other

  5. (11)

    Do you have any other comments or anything else you would like to share?

________________________________________

Thank you for your contribution to our research!

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Farrar, J., King, T. To Punish or Not to Punish? The Impact of Tax Fraud Punishment on Observers’ Tax Compliance. J Bus Ethics 183, 289–311 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-022-05061-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-022-05061-w

Keywords

Navigation