Abstract
In business settings, decision makers facing moral issues often experience the challenges of continuous changes. This dynamic process has been less examined in previous literature on moral decision making. We borrow theories on learning strategies and computational models from decision neuroscience to explain the updating and learning mechanisms underlying moral decision processes. Specifically, we present two main learning strategies: model-free learning, wherein the values of choices are updated in a trial-and-error fashion sustaining the formation of habits and model-based learning, wherein the brain updates more general cognitive maps and associations, thus sustaining flexible and state-dependent behaviors. We then summarize studies explaining the neuro-computational processes of both learning strategies—the calculation of prediction errors and valuation. We conclude by emphasizing how the incorporation of dynamic aspects in moral decision making could open new avenues for understanding moral behaviors in a changing world.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Notes
Notice that in this context, the term “value” refers to the magnitude of outcome and should not be confused with other interpretations such as social values etc.
The concept of utility here is more generic than the way is used by “Utilitarian” approaches.
Abbreviations
- DN:
-
Decision neuroscience
- RPE:
-
Reward prediction error
- SPE:
-
State prediction error
- VMPFC:
-
Ventro-medial prefrontac cortex
- SMH:
-
Somatic marker hypothesis
- DLPFC:
-
Dorso lateral prefrontal cortex
References
Abbink, K., Irlenbusch, B., & Renner, E. (2002). An experimental bribery game. Journal of Law Economics and Organization, 18(2), 428–454.
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32(3), 537–551.
Aimone, J. A., Houser, D., & Weber, B. (2014). Neural signatures of betrayal aversion: An fMRI study of trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1782), 20132127.
Alessi, S., & Petry, N. (2003). Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behavioural Processes, 64(3), 345–354.
Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(11), 1032–1037.
Ariely, D., Bracha, A., & Meier, S. (2009). Doing good or doing well? Image motivation and monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. American Economic Review, 99(1), 544–555.
Baker, F., Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2003). Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: Similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(3), 382.
Ballard, K., Knutson, B. (2009). Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. NeuroImage, 45, 143–150.
Bayer, J., Bandurski, P., & Sommer, T. (2013). Differential modulation of activity related to the anticipation of monetary gains and losses across the menstrual cycle. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38(10), 3519–3526.
Bayer, H. M., & Glimcher, P. W. (2005). Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron, 47(1), 129–141.
Bechara, A., & Damasio, H. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part I): Impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1675–1689.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: Some questions and answers. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(4), 159–162. discussion 162–154.
Behrens, T. E., Hunt, L. T., Woolrich, M. W., & Rushworth, M. F. (2008). Associative learning of social value. Nature, 456(7219), 245–249.
Beierholm, U. R., Anen, C., Quartz, S., & Bossaerts, P. (2011). Separate encoding of model-based and model-free valuations in the human brain. Neuroimage, 58(3), 955–962.
Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 146(4), 447–454.
Bickel, W. K., Pitcock, J. A., Yi, R., & Angtuaco, E. J. (2009). Congruence of BOLD response across intertemporal choice conditions: Fictive and real money gains and losses. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(27), 8839–8846.
Bossaerts, P. (2009). What decision neuroscience teaches us about financial decision making. Annual Review of Financial Economics, 1, 383–404.
Burke, C. J., & Tobler, P. N. (2011). Reward skewness coding in the insula independent of probability and loss. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(5), 2415–2422.
Burke, C. J., Tobler, P. N., Baddeley, M., & Schultz, W. (2010). Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(32), 14431–14436.
Camerer, C. F. (2008). Neuroeconomics: Opening the gray box. Neuron, 60(3), 416–419.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control-theory: A useful conceptual-framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92(1), 111–135.
Christopoulos, G., & Hong, Y. Y. (2013). Turning two uninvited guests into prominent speakers: Toward a dynamic culture neuroscience. Psychological Inquiry, 24(1), 20–25.
Christopoulos, G. I., & King-Casas, B. (2015). With you or against you: Social orientation dependent learning signals guide actions made for others. Neuroimage, 104, 326–335.
Christopoulos, G., Kokkinaki, F., Harvey, N., & Sevdalis, N. (2011). Paying for no reason? (Mis-) perceptions of product attributes in separate vs. joint product evaluation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32(5), 857–864.
Christopoulos, G. I., Tobler, P. N., Bossaerts, P., Dolan, R. J., & Schultz, W. (2009). Neural correlates of value, risk, and risk aversion contributing to decision making under risk. The Journal of neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(40), 12574–12583.
Chung, D., Christopoulos, G. I., King-Casas, B., Ball, S. B., & Chiu, P. H. (2015). Social signals of safety and risk confer utility and have asymmetric effects on observers’ choices. Nature Neuroscience, 18(6), 912–916.
Cohen, J. D., & Aston-Jones, G. (2005). Cognitive neuroscience: Decision amid uncertainty. Nature, 436(7050), 471–472.
Cole, M., & Packer, M. (2015). A bio-cultural-historical approach to the study of development. In M. J. Gelfand, C. Y. Chiu, & Y. Y. Hong (Eds.), Advances in culture and psychology (Vol. 6). New York: Oxford University Press.
Cushman, F., Young, L., & Hauser, M. (2006). The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1082–1089.
Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 351(1346), 1413–1420.
Daw, N. D. (2012). Model-based reinforcement learning as cognitive search: Neurocomputational theories. In P. M. Todd & T. R. Robbins (Eds.), Cognitive search: Evolution, algorithms and the brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Daw, N. D., & Doya, K. (2006). The computational neurobiology of learning and reward. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 199–204.
Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron, 69(6), 1204–1215.
Daw, N. D., Niv, Y., & Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience, 8(12), 1704–1711.
Dayan, P., & Daw, N. D. (2008). Decision theory, reinforcement learning, and the brain. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(4), 429–453.
Dayan, P., Niv, Y., Seymour, B., & Daw, N. D. (2006). The misbehavior of value and the discipline of the will. Neural Networks, 19(8), 1153–1160.
de Araujo, I. E., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., & McGlone, F. (2003). Human cortical responses to water in the mouth, and the effects of thirst. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(3), 1865–1876.
de Araujo, I. E., Rolls, E. T., Velazco, M. I., Margot, C., & Cayeux, I. (2005). Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron, 46(4), 671–679.
de Wit, H., Enggasser, J. L., & Richards, J. B. (2002). Acute administration of d-amphetamine decreases impulsivity in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(5), 813–825.
Decety, J., Jackson, P. L., Sommerville, J. A., Chaminade, T., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2004). The neural bases of cooperation and competition: An fMRI investigation. Neuroimage, 23(2), 744–751.
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005a). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1611–1618.
Delgado, M. R., Miller, M. M., Inati, S., & Phelps, E. A. (2005b). An fMRI study of reward-related probability learning. Neuroimage, 24(3), 862–873.
Deserno, L., Huys, Q. J. M., Boehme, R., Buchert, R., Heinze, H. J., Grace, A. A., et al. (2015). Ventral striatal dopamine reflects behavioral and neural signatures of model-based control during sequential decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(5), 1595–1600.
Doya, K. (2002). Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Networks, 15(4–6), 495–506.
Doya, K., Samejima, K., Katagiri, K., & Kawato, M. (2002). Multiple model-based reinforcement learning. Neural Computation, 14(6), 1347–1369.
Dreher, J. C., & Burnod, Y. (2002). An integrative theory of the phasic and tonic modes of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Neural Networks, 15(4–6), 583–602.
Du, W., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2002). Cross-cultural comparisons of discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Record, 52(4), 479–492.
Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Kosfeld, M. (2005). Neuroeconomic foundations of trust and social preferences: Initial evidence. American Economic Review, 95(2), 346–351.
Fermin, A., Yoshida, T., Ito, M., Yoshimoto, J., & Doya, K. (2010). Evidence for model-based action planning in a sequential finger movement task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 42(6), 371–379.
Fisher, H., Aron, A., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Romantic love: An fMRI study of a neural mechanism for mate choice. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 58–62.
Fliessbach, K., Weber, B., Trautner, P., Dohmen, T., Sunde, U., Elger, C. E., & Falk, A. (2007). Social comparison affects reward-related brain activity in the human ventral striatum. Science, 318(5854), 1305–1308.
Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351–401.
Fuster, A., & Meier, S. (2010). Another hidden cost of incentives: The detrimental effect on norm enforcement. Management Science, 56(1), 57–70.
Garrett, T. A., & Sobel, R. S. (1999). Gamblers favor skewness, not risk: Further evidence from United States’ lottery games. Economics Letters, 63(1), 85–90.
Gillan, C. M., Otto, A. R., Phelps, E. A., & Daw, N. D. (2015). Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 523.
Gillan, C. M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B. J., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & de Wit, S. (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(7), 718–726.
Glascher, J., Daw, N., Dayan, P., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). States versus rewards: Dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. Neuron, 66(4), 585–595.
Glimcher, P. W., Dorris, M. C., & Bayer, H. M. (2005). Physiological utility theory and the neuroeconomics of choice. Games and Economic Behavior, 52(2), 213–256.
Glimcher, P. W., & Fehr, E. (2013). Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain. London: Academic Press.
Gneezy, U., Meier, S., & Rey-Biel, P. (2011). When and why incentives (Don’t) work to modify behavior. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 25(4), 191–209.
Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). A fine is a price. Journal of Legal Studies, 29(1), 1–17.
Gong, M., Baron, J., & Kunreuther, H. (2009). Group cooperation under uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 39(3), 251–270.
Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5(1), 33–36.
Greene, J. D. (2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 322–323. author reply 323–324.
Greene, J. D. (2009). The cognitive neuroscience of moral judgment. The Cognitive Neurosciences, 4, 987–999.
Greene, J. D., Cushman, F. A., Stewart, L. E., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Pushing moral buttons: The interaction between personal force and intention in moral judgment. Cognition, 111(3), 364–371.
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.
Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 998–1002.
Haruno, M., & Kawato, M. (2006). Different neural correlates of reward expectation and reward expectation error in the putamen and caudate nucleus during stimulus-action-reward association learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95(2), 948–959.
Harvey, A. H., Kirk, U., Denfield, G. H., & Montague, P. R. (2010). Monetary favors and their influence on neural responses and revealed preference. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(28), 9597–9602.
Heil, S. H., Johnson, M. W., Higgins, S. T., & Bickel, W. K. (2006). Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. Addictive Behaviors, 31(7), 1290–1294.
Hollerman, J. R., Tremblay, L., & Schultz, W. (1998). Influence of reward expectation on behavior-related neuronal activity in primate striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(2), 947–963.
Hong, Y. Y. (2008). Dynamic constructivist approach to culture. International Journal of Psychology, 43(3–4), 421–422.
Huettel, S. A., Song, A. W., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Decisions under uncertainty: Probabilistic context influences activation of prefrontal and parietal cortices. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 25(13), 3304–3311.
Ikeda, S., Kang, M.-I., & Ohtake, F. (2010). Hyperbolic discounting, the sign effect, and the body mass index. Journal of Health Economics, 29(2), 268–284.
Illes, J., & Bird, S. J. (2006). Neuroethics: A modern context for ethics in neuroscience. Trends in Neurosciences, 29(9), 511–517.
Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., & Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron, 58(2), 284–294.
Jiang, T., Soussignan, R., Schaal, B., & Royet, J. P. (2014). Reward for food odors: An fMRI study of liking and wanting as a function of metabolic state and BMI. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.
Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision-making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 366–395.
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment, 49
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2005). A model of heuristic judgment. In The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp 267–293.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: Analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291.
King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R., & Montague, P. R. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science, 308(5718), 78–83.
Knutson, B., Wimmer, G. E., Kuhnen, C. M., & Winkielman, P. (2008). Nucleus accumbens activation mediates the influence of reward cues on financial risk taking. NeuroReport, 19(5), 509–513.
Kringelbach, M. L., O’Doherty, J., Rolls, E. T., & Andrews, C. (2003). Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cerebral Cortex, 13(10), 1064–1071.
Krueger, F., McCabe, K., Moll, J., Kriegeskorte, N., Zahn, R., Strenziok, M., et al. (2007). Neural correlates of trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(50), 20084–20089.
Kuhnen, C. M., & Knutson, B. (2005). The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron, 47(5), 763–770.
Kunreuther, H., Silvasi, G., Bradlow, E. T., & Small, D. (2009). Bayesian analysis of deterministic and stochastic prisoner’s dilemma games. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(5), 363–384.
Lacey, S., Hagtvedt, H., Patrick, V. M., Anderson, A., Stilla, R., Deshpande, G., et al. (2011). Art for reward’s sake: Visual art recruits the ventral striatum. Neuroimage, 55(1), 420–433.
Lauharatanahirun, N., Christopoulos, G. I., & King-Casas, B. (2012). Neural computations underlying social risk sensitivity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 213.
Lawrence, N. S., Hinton, E. C., Parkinson, J. A., & Lawrence, A. D. (2012). Nucleus accumbens response to food cues predicts subsequent snack consumption in women and increased body mass index in those with reduced self-control. Neuroimage, 63(1), 415–422.
Lee, S. W., Shimojo, S., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2014). Neural computations underlying arbitration between model-based and model-free learning. Neuron, 81(3), 687–699.
Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34(34), 199–249.
Loewenstein, G. (1987). Anticipation and the valuation of delayed consumption. Economic Journal, 97(387), 666–684.
Loewenstein, G. F. (1988). Frames of mind in intertemporal choice. Management Science, 34(2), 200–214.
Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2004). Animal spirits: Affective and deliberative processes in economic behavior. Available at SSRN 539843.
McClure, S. M., Berns, G. S., & Montague, P. R. (2003). Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron, 38(2), 339–346.
McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., & Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron, 44(2), 379–387.
McCoy, A. N., & Platt, M. L. (2005). Expectations and outcomes: Decision-making in the primate brain. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 191(3), 201–211.
Mischel, W., Grusec, J., & Masters, J. C. (1969). Effects of expected delay time on subjective value of rewards and punishments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11(4), 363–373.
Miyapuram, K. P., & Pammi, V. S. (2013). Understanding decision neuroscience: A multidisciplinary perspective and neural substrates. Progress in Brain Research, 202, 239–266.
Miyapuram, K. P., Tobler, P. N., Gregorios-Pippas, L., & Schultz, W. (2012). BOLD responses in reward regions to hypothetical and imaginary monetary rewards. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1692–1699.
Moll, J., & de Oliveira-Souza, R. (2007). Response to Greene: Moral sentiments and reason: Friends or foes? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 323.
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2002a). Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. Neuroimage, 16(3 Pt 1), 696–703.
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mourao-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., & Pessoa, L. (2002b). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(7), 2730–2736.
Moll, J., Eslinger, P. J., & Oliveira-Souza, R. (2001). Frontopolar and anterior temporal cortex activation in a moral judgment task: Preliminary functional MRI results in normal subjects. Arq Neuropsiquiatr, 59(3-B), 657–664.
Montague, P. R., Dolan, R. J., Friston, K. J., & Dayan, P. (2012). Computational psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 72–80.
Montague, P. R., Harvey, A. H., & Kirk, U. (2014). Using fMRI to study valuation and choice.
Montague, P. R., & King-Casas, B. (2007). Efficient statistics, common currencies and the problem of reward-harvesting. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(12), 514–519.
Mulder, L. B., & Aquino, K. (2013). The role of moral identity in the aftermath of dishonesty. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121(2), 219–230.
Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay discounting: Trait variable? Behavioural Processes, 87(1), 1–9.
Odum, A. L., Madden, G. J., & Bickel, W. K. (2002). Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 4(3), 295–303.
Osuch, E. A., Bluhm, R. L., Williamson, P. C., Theberge, J., Densmore, M., & Neufeld, R. W. (2009). Brain activation to favorite music in healthy controls and depressed patients. NeuroReport, 20(13), 1204–1208.
Paloyelis, Y., Asherson, P., Mehta, M. A., Faraone, S. V., & Kuntsi, J. (2010). DAT1 and COMT effects on delay discounting and trait impulsivity in male adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(12), 2414–2426.
Pavlov, I. P. (2003). Conditioned reflexes. New York: Courier Corporation.
Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature, 442(7106), 1042–1045.
Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., & Rangel, A. (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(37), 9984–9988.
Platt, M. L., & Huettel, S. A. (2008). Risky business: The neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 398–403.
Preuschoff, K., Quartz, S. R., & Bossaerts, P. (2008). Human insula activation reflects risk prediction errors as well as risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 2745–2752.
Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 545–556.
Reynolds, S. J. (2006). A neurocognitive model of the ethical decision-making process: Implications for study and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 737–748.
Rilling, J., Gutman, D., Zeh, T., Pagnoni, G., Berns, G., & Kilts, C. (2002). A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron, 35(2), 395–405.
Robertson, D., Snarey, J., Ousley, O., Harenski, K., DuBois Bowman, F., Gilkey, R., & Kilts, C. (2007). The neural processing of moral sensitivity to issues of justice and care. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 755–766.
Roberts, A. C., Christopoulos, G. I., Car, J., Soh, C. K., & Lu, M. (2016). Psycho-biological factors associated with underground spaces: What can the new era of cognitive neuroscience offer to their study? Tunnelling and Underground Space Technology.
Rothschild, M., & Stiglitz, J. E. (1970). Increasing Risk. 1. Definition. Journal of Economic Theory, 2(3), 225–243.
Sanfey, A. G. (2007). Social decision-making: Insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science, 318(5850), 598–602.
Schonberg, T., Daw, N. D., Joel, D., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(47), 12860–12867.
Schultz, W. (2008). Introduction. Neuroeconomics: The promise and the profit. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 363(1511), 3767–3769.
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 1593–1599.
Schultz, W., Preuschoff, K., Camerer, C., Hsu, M., Fiorillo, C. D., Tobler, P. N., & Bossaerts, P. (2008). Explicit neural signals reflecting reward uncertainty. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 363(1511), 3801–3811.
Schultz, W., Tremblay, L., & Hollerman, J. R. (1998). Reward prediction in primate basal ganglia and frontal cortex. Neuropharmacology, 37(4–5), 421–429.
Schultz, W., Tremblay, L., & Hollerman, J. R. (2003). Changes in behavior-related neuronal activity in the striatum during learning. Trends in Neurosciences, 26(6), 321–328.
Small, D. M., Gregory, M. D., Mak, Y. E., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M. M., & Parrish, T. (2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron, 39(4), 701–711.
Smith, D. V., & Huettel, S. A. (2010). Decision neuroscience: Neuroeconomics. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(6), 854–871.
Spinella, M., Lester, D., & Yang, B. (2004). Gambling and delaying rewards as a function of frontal system dysfunction: A study in neuroeconomics. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99(3 Pt 1), 993–994.
Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80(1), 28–44.
Sutton, R., & Barto, A. (1988). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Suzuki, S., Harasawa, N., Ueno, K., Gardner, J. L., Ichinohe, N., Haruno, M., et al. (2012). Learning to simulate others’ decisions. Neuron, 74(6), 1125–1137.
Symmonds, M., Wright, N. D., Bach, D. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Deconstructing risk: Separable encoding of variance and skewness in the brain. Neuroimage, 58(4), 1139–1149.
Tanaka, S. C., Yamada, K., Yoneda, H., & Ohtake, F. (2014). Neural mechanisms of gain-loss asymmetry in temporal discounting. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(16), 5595–5602.
Thaler, R. (1981a). Some empirical-evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8(3), 201–207.
Thaler, R. H. (1981b). Maximization and self-control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(3), 403–404.
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan.
Tobler, P. N., Christopoulos, G. I., O’Doherty, J. P., Dolan, R. J., & Schultz, W. (2008a). Neuronal distortions of reward probability without choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(45), 11703–11711.
Tobler, P. N., Kalis, A., & Kalenscher, T. (2008b). The role of moral utility in decision making: An interdisciplinary framework. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(4), 390–401.
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55(4), 189–208.
Venkatraman, V. (2013). Why bother with the brain? A role for decision neuroscience in understanding strategic variability. Progress in Brain Research, 202, 267–288.
Voon, V., Derbyshire, K., Ruck, C., Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Enander, J., et al. (2015). Disorders of compulsivity: A common bias towards learning habits. Molecular Psychiatry, 20(3), 345–352.
Winstanley, C. A., Dalley, J. W., Theobald, D. E., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Global 5-HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay-discounting task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 170(3), 320–331.
Wunderlich, K., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2012a). Mapping value based planning and extensively trained choice in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 786–791.
Wunderlich, K., Smittenaar, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2012b). Dopamine enhances model-based over model-free choice behavior. Neuron, 75(3), 418–424.
Yates, J. F., & Watts, R. A. (1975). Preferences for deferred losses. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13(2), 294–306.
Young, L., & Koenigs, M. (2007). Investigating emotion in moral cognition: A review of evidence from functional neuroimaging and neuropsychology. British Medical Bulletin, 84, 69–79.
Zink, C. F., Pagnoni, G., Martin-Skurski, M. E., Chappelow, J. C., & Berns, G. S. (2004). Human striatal responses to monetary reward depend on saliency. Neuron, 42(3), 509–517.
Zink, C. F., Tong, Y., Chen, Q., Bassett, D. S., Stein, J. L., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2008). Know your place: Neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron, 58(2), 273–283.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the Editors as well as two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments in an earlier version of this paper. The preparation of this article was partially supported by the Academic Research Fund (AcRF) Tier 2 (MOE2012-T2-1-051) of the Ministry of Education, Singapore, awarded to Ying-yi Hong and George Christopoulos and by the Academic Research Fund (AcRF) Tier 1 (RG 1/11 M4010946.010) of the Ministry of Education, Singapore, awarded to the first author.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Christopoulos, G.I., Liu, XX. & Hong, Yy. Toward an Understanding of Dynamic Moral Decision Making: Model-Free and Model-Based Learning. J Bus Ethics 144, 699–715 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3058-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3058-1