Biology & Philosophy

, Volume 29, Issue 5, pp 699–710 | Cite as

Feeling the future: prospects for a theory of implicit prospection

Article

Abstract

Mental time travel refers to the ability of an organism to project herself backward and forward in time, using episodic memory and imagination to simulate past and future experiences. The evolution of mental time travel gives humans a unique capacity for prospection: the ability to pre-experience the future. Discussions of mental time travel treat it as an instance of explicit prospection. We argue that implicit simulations of past and future experience can also be used as a way of gaining information about the future to shape preferences and guide behaviour.

Keywords

Mental time travel Somatic marker hypothesis Implicit processes Iowa gambling task Prospection 

References

  1. Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL (2007) Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45(7):1363–1377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bauer RM (1984) Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: a neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test. Neuropsychologia 22:457–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bauer RM (1986) The cognitive psychophysiology of prosopagnosia. In: Ellis HD, Jeeves MA, Newcombe F, Young A (eds) Aspects of face processing. Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, pp 253–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1996) Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 6(2):215–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bowman CH, Evans CE, Turnbull OH (2005) Artificial time constraints on the Iowa gambling task: the effects on behavioural performance and subjective experience. Brain Cognit 57(1):21–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brass M, Heyes C (2005) Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends Cognit Sci 9(10):489–495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Breen N, Caine D, Coltheart M, Hendy J, Roberts C (2000) Towards an understanding of delusions of misidentification. Mind Lang 15:74–110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Breen N, Coltheart M, Caine D (2001) A two-way window on face recognition. Trends Cognit Sci 5:234–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cognit Sci 11(2):49–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Busby J, Suddendorf T (2005) Recalling yesterday and predicting tomorrow. Cognit Dev 20:362–372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cognit Sci 12(5):187–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Christoff K, Gordon AM, Smallwood J, Smith R, Schooler JW (2009) Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106(21):8719–8724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395(6699):272–274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clayton NS, Bussey TJ, Dickinson A (2003) Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nat Rev Neurosci 4:685–691CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Crone EA, Somsen RJ, Van Beek B, Van Der Molen MW (2004) Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecendents and consequences of decision making. Psychophysiology 41(4):531–540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2006) Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science 312(5780):1662–1665CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H (1991) Somatic markers and the guidance of behaviour: theory and preliminary testing. In: Levin HS, Eisenberg HM, Benton AL (eds) Frontal lobe function and dysfunction. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 217–229Google Scholar
  18. Davies M (1987) Tacit knowledge and semantic theory mind. Mind 96:441–462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. De Haan EHF, Young A, Newcombe F (1987a) Faces interfere with name classification in a prosopagnosic patient. Cortex 2:309–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. De Haan EHF, Young A, Newcombe F (1987b) Face recognition without awareness. Cognit Neuropsychol 4:385–415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Degonda N, Mondadori CR, Bosshardt S, Schmidt CF, Boesiger P, Nitsch RM, Hock C, Henke K (2005) Implicit associative learning engages the hippocampus and interacts with explicit associative learning. Neuron 46(3):505–520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Delazer M, Benke T (1999) Arithmetic reasoning and implicit memory. Brain Cognit 40:94–97Google Scholar
  23. Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Lawrence AD (2006) The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 239–271(30):239–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Miezin FM, Barch DM, Schlaggar BL (2008) The maturing architecture of the brain’s default network. Proc Natl Acad Sci 105(10):4028–4032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM (2008) Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol Bull 134(1):31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V (2003) Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci 100(1):253–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hassabis D, Maguire EA (2007) Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cognit Sci 11(7):299–306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hauser MD (2006) The liver and the moral organ. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 1(3):214–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Heyes CM (1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behav Brain Sci 21(1):101–114Google Scholar
  30. Heyes C (2010) Where do mirror neurons come from? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34(4):575–583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Koechlin E, Summerfield C (2007) An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cognit Sci 11(6):229–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Leslie AM (1994) Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition 50:211–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Maia TV, McClelland JL (2004) A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101(45):16075–16080CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Maia TV, McClelland JL (2005) The somatic marker hypothesis: still many questions but no answers. Trends Cognit Sci 9(4):162–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315(5810):393–395CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Roberts WA, Feeney MC (2009) The comparative study of mental time travel. Trends Cognit Sci 13(6):271–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ruffman T (2000) Nonverbal theory of mind: is it important, is it implicit, is it simulation, is it relevant to autism. In Astington JW (ed) Minds in the making: essays in honor of David R. Olson. pp 250–266Google Scholar
  38. Sarnecka BW, Carey S (2008) How counting represents number: what children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition 108(3):662–674CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schacter DL, Addis DR (2007) The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos Trans Royal Soc B: Biol Sci 362(1481):773–786CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 8(9):657–661CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Schweinberger SR, Burton AM (2003) Covert recognition and the neural system for face processing. Cortex 39(1):9–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Shettleworth SJ (2010) Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends Cognit Sci 14(11):477–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim AS (2009) The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cognit Neurosci 21(3):489–510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sterelny K (2011) From hominins to humans: how sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philos Trans Royal Soc B: Biol Sci 366(1566):809–822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Stuss DT, Alexander MP (2000) Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol Res 63(3–4):289–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Beha Brain Sci 30(3):299–312Google Scholar
  47. Suddendorf T, Addis DR, Corballis MC (2009) Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind. Philos Trans Royal Soc B: Biol Sci 364(1521):1317–1324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Szpunar KK (2010) Episodic future thought an emerging concept. Perspect Psychol Sci 5(2):142–162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tomb I, Hauser M, Deldin P, Caramazza A (2002) Do somatic markers mediate decisions on the gambling task? Nature Neuroscience 5(11), 1103–1104 (author reply 1104)Google Scholar
  50. Tranel D, Damasio AR (1988a) Knowledge without awareness: an autonomic index of facial recognition in prosopagnosics. Science 228:1453–1454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Tranel D, Damasio AR (1988b) Non-conscious face recognition in patients with face agnosia. Beha Brain Res 30:235–249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1995) Double dissociation between overt and covert recognition. J Cognit Neurosci 7:425–432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Young AW (1994) Covert recognition. In: Farah M, Ratcligg G (eds) The neuropsychology of high-level vision: collected tutorial essays. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 331–358Google Scholar
  54. Zelazo PD, Frye D (1998) Cognitive complexity and control: II. The development of executive function in childhood. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 7(4):121–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Swiss Centre for Affective SciencesUniversity of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland
  2. 2.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of AdelaideAdelaideAustralia
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations