Skip to main content
Log in

Learning, evolvability and exploratory behaviour: extending the evolutionary reach of learning

  • Published:
Biology & Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Traditional accounts of the role of learning in evolution have concentrated upon its capacity as a source of fitness to individuals. In this paper I use a case study from invasive species biology—the role of conditioned taste aversion in mitigating the impact of cane toads on the native species of Northern Australia—to highlight a role for learning beyond this—as a source of evolvability to populations. This has two benefits. First, it highlights an otherwise under-appreciated role for learning in evolution that does not rely on social learning as an inheritance channel nor “special” evolutionary processes such as genetic accommodation (both of which many are skeptical about). Second, and more significantly, it makes clear important and interesting parallels between learning and exploratory behaviour in development. These parallels motivate the applicability of results from existing research into learning and learning evolution to our understanding the evolution of evolvability more generally.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There are exceptions to this general rule. A small number of native species have been observed to be relatively “immune” to toad toxin (e.g. the keelback snake, Tropidonophis mairii). The “preadaptation” to toads in these lineages is believed to be the product of contact between ancestors of these native species and Bufonid toads in Asia (where such species are endemic) prior to migration onto the Australian continent (Llewelyn et al. 2010a, 2011).

  2. Note, red-bellied black snakes are not incapable of learning full stop, they simply do not exhibit learning with respect to cane toads (Phillips and Shine 2006).

  3. It is important to note that this claim—that learning influences evolvability—is made elsewhere (albeit in passing) within the literature on the contribution of adaptive phenotypic plasticity to the evolvability of populations (in particular, see Pfennig et al. (2010); West-Eberhard (2003); Fitzpatrick (2012)) and also that concerning the Baldwin effect (e.g., Papineau 2005). Here, I am both making the claim regarding learning clearer and more specific but also, as will become apparent, highlighting some novel implications for evolutionary developmental biology.

  4. Note that, although there are documented cases of relatively large, but genetically homogeneous, populations, such populations are rare and fragile. To illustrate, prior to 2008, Tasmanian devil (Sarcophilus harrisii) populations in Tasmania were very large, but exhibited low levels of genetic diversity at a number of particularly polymorphic parts of the mammalian genome (McCallum 2008). Therefore, this population lacked robustness. Since 2008, devil populations have been decimated by a particularly virulent host-specific pathogen whose transmission is aided by the genetic homogeneity of the devil population. Because of the fragility of populations like this (and thus their rarity), it is a relatively uncontroversial assumption of much of conservation biology that small populations have lower standing genetic variation than larger ones (Boyce 1992; Shaffer 1981).

  5. There is considerable conceptual confusion surrounding the term "robustness" both within the sciences and in the philosophy of science (Wimsatt 2007) but also more specifically in evolutionary developmental biology (this confusion concerns relationship between robustness and evolvability—see Wagner 2005; Wagner 2008; Lenski et al. 2006; Brookfield 2009). As such, note that here I mean “robustness” to refer to the ability of a population or lineage to respond adaptively to environmental change. I take it that, at least with respect to a population’s capacity to evolve adaptation (i.e., their evolvability with respect to adaptation), this type of robustness is not in conflict with evolvability. A population that is robust in this manner is also evolvable with respect to complex adaptation.

  6. Unfortunately, I was unable to find any data in the literature on the genetic diversity of red-bellied black snake and planigale populations that I could use to test the hypotheses I make here.

  7. There are certain situations (e.g., chimerism) in which there can be divergence in the interests of the cells within the body but these are rare.

  8. I was able to find some research that is at least related. In birds there is evidence that there is no link between behavioural flexibility and extinction risk (Nicolakakis et al. 2003). Behavioural flexibility and asocial learning are not strictly the same thing, however, though the former may be necessary for the latter. It would be interesting to see if there was a relationship between behavioural flexibility and extinction risk when coupled with asocial learning.

References

  • Barrett RDH, Schluter D (2008) Adaptation from standing genetic variation. Trends Ecol Evol 23(1):38–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beeton RJS, Buckley KI, Jones GJ, Morgan D, Reichelt RE, Trewin D (2006) Australia state of the environment 2006. Australian Government: Department of Sustainability, Environment, Water, Population and Communities, Australia

    Google Scholar 

  • Begon M, Harper JL, Townsend CR (1986) Ecology. Individuals, populations and communities, 3rd edn. Blackwell Science Ltd, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyce MS (1992) Population viability analysis. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 23:481–506

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breeden K (1963) Cane toad (Bufo marinus). Wildl Aust 1:31

    Google Scholar 

  • Brigandt I (forthcoming), From developmental constraint to evolvability: how concepts figure in explanation and disciplinary identity. In: Love AC (ed) Conceptual change in biology: scientific and philosophical perspectives of evolution and development. Springer, Berlin

  • Brookfield JFY (2009) Evolution and evolvability: celebrating Darwin 200. Biol Lett 5(1):44–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown RL (forthcoming) What evolvability really is. Br J Philos Sci

  • Burnett S (1997) Colonising cane toads cause population declines in native predators: reliable anecdotal information and management implications. Pac Conserv Biol 3(1):65–72

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron E (2011) Cane toad factsheet, trans. 9 April (Australian Museum)

  • Carroll SP (2007a) Natives adapting to invasive species: ecology, genes, and the sustainability of conservation. Ecol Res 22(6):892–901

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll SP (2007b) Brave new world: the epistatic foundations of natives adapting to invaders. Genetica 129(2):193–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen KK, Kovaříková A (1967) Pharmacology and toxicology of cane toad venom. J Pharm Sci 56(12):1535–1541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Covacevich J, Archer M (1975) The distribution of the cane toad, Bufo marinus, in Australia and its effects on indigenous vertebrates. Mem Qld Mus 17(2):305–310

    Google Scholar 

  • Crossland MR (2001) Ability of predatory native Australian fishes to learn to avoid toxic larvae of the introduced cane toad Bufo marinus. J Fish Biol 59(2):319–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crossland MR, Brown GP, Anstis M, Shilton CM, Shine R (2008) Mass mortality of native anuran tadpoles in tropical Australia due to the invasive cane toad (Bufo marinus). Biol Conserv 141(9):2387–2394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong G (2005) Evolution of phenotypic plasticity: patterns of plasticity and the emergence of ecotypes. New Phytol 166:101–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doody JS, Green B, Sims R, Rhind D, West P, Steer D (2006) Indirect impacts of invasive cane toads (Bufo marinus) on nest predation in pig-nosed turtles (Carettochelys insculpta). Wildl Res 33(5):349–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald M (1990) Rattus rattus: the introduced black rat, a successful predator on the introduced cane toad Bufo marinus in northern New South Wales. Herpetofauna 20:9–14

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitzpatrick BM (2012) Underappreciated consequences of phenotypic plasticity for ecological speciation. Int J Ecol 2012:1–12

  • Freeland WJ (1986) Populations of cane toad, Bufo-Marinus, in relation to time since colonization. Wildl Res 13(2):321–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeland WJ (1990) Effects of the cane toad Bufo marinus on native Australian wildlife: a review of past and current research (CCNT, unpublished report)

  • Galef BG (1992) The question of animal culture. Hum Nat 3(2):157–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galef BG (2009) Culture in animals? In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 222–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia J, Ervin FR, Koelling RA (1966) Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychon Sci 5(3):121–122

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhart J, Kirschner M (2007) The theory of facilitated variation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104(Suppl 1):8582–8589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenlees MJ, Phillips BL, Shine R (2010a) Adjusting to a toxic invader: native Australian frogs learn not to prey on cane toads. Behav Ecol 21(5):966–971

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenlees MJ, Phillips BL, Shine R (2010b) An invasive species imposes selection on life-history traits of a native frog. Biol J Linn Soc 100(2):329–336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartfelder K (2005) The phenotype strikes back and is out on more than one limb: a review of Developmental Plasticity an Evolution, by Mary Jane West-Ebherhard. Genet Mol Biol 28(3):475–478

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM (1993) Imitation, culture and cognition. Anim Behav 46(5):999–1010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes C (2011) What’s social about social learning? J Comp Pyschol 126(2):193–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoekstra HE, Coyne JA (2007) The locus of evolution: evo devo and the genetics of adaptation. Evolution 61(5):995–1016

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houle D (1992) Comparing evolvability and variability of quantitative traits. Genetics 130(1):195–204

    Google Scholar 

  • Kearney M, Phillips BL, Tracy CR, Christian KA, Betts G, Porter WP (2008) Modeling species distributions without using species distributions: the cane toad in Australia under current and future climates. Ecography 31(4):423–434

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner MW, Gerhart JC (2006) The plausibility of life: resolving Darwin’s dilemma. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Hoppitt W (2003) Do animals have culture? Evolut Anthropol Issues News Rev 12(3):150–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Janik VM (2006) The animal cultures debate. Trends Ecol Evol 21(10):542–547

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Brown C, Krause J (2003) Learning in fishes: from three-second memory to culture. Fish Fish 4(3):199–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Kendal JR, Kendal RL (2009) Animal culture: problems and solutions. In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 174–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Lampo M, De Leo GA (1998) The invasion ecology of the cane toad Bufo marinus: from South America to Australia. Ecol Appl 8(2):388–396

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenski RE, Barrick JE, Ofria C (2006) Balancing robustness and evolvability. PLoS Biol 4(12):e428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Letnic M, Webb JK, Shine R (2008) Invasive cane toads (Bufo marinus) cause mass mortality of freshwater crocodiles (Crocodylus johnstoni) in tropical Australia. Biol Conserv 141(7):1773–1782

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lever C (2001) The cane toad: the history and ecology of a successful colonist. Westbury Academic and Scientific Publishing, Yorkshire

    Google Scholar 

  • Llewelyn J, Schwarzkopf L, Alford R, Shine R (2010a) Something different for dinner? Responses of a native Australian predator (the keelback snake) to an invasive prey species (the cane toad). Biol Invasions 12(5):1045–1051

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Llewelyn J, Webb JK, Schwarzkopf L, Alford R, Shine R (2010b) Behavioural responses of carnivorous marsupials (Planigale maculata) to toxic invasive cane toads (Bufo marinus). Austral Ecol 35(5):560–567

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Llewelyn J, Phillips BL, Brown GP, Schwarzkopf L, Alford RA, Shine R (2011) Adaptation or preadaptation: why are keelback snakes (Tropidonophis mairii) less vulnerable to invasive cane toads (Bufo marinus) than are other Australian snakes? Evol Ecol 25(1):13–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe S, Browne M, Boudjelas S, De Poorter M (2000) 100 of the world’s most invasive alien species: a selection from the global invasive species database. Invasive Species Specialist Group, Auckland

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz B (1971) Venomous cane toads and frogs. In: Bucherl W, Buckley EE (eds) Venomous animals and their venoms, vol 2. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason JR, Reidinger RF (1982) Observational learning of food aversions in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus), Auk 99(3):548–554

    Google Scholar 

  • McCallum H (2008) Tasmanian devil facial tumour disease: lessons for conservation biology. Trends Ecol Evol 23(11):631–637

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mery F, Kawecki TJ (2002) Experimental evolution of learning ability in fruit flies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99(22):14274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolakakis N, Sol D, Lefebvre L (2003) Behavioural flexibility predicts species richness in birds, but not extinction risk. Anim Behav 65(3):445–452

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papineau D (2005) Social learning and the Baldwin Effect. In: Zilhao A (ed) Social learning and the Baldwin Effect. Routledge, Oxon, pp 40–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfennig DW, Wund MA, Snell-Rood EC, Cruickshank T, Schlichting CD, Moczek AP (2010) Phenotypic plasticity’s impacts on diversification and speciation. Trends Ecol Evol 25:459–467

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips BL, Shine R (2004) Adapting to an invasive species: toxic cane toads induce morphological change in Australian snakes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101(49):17150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips BL, Shine R (2006) An invasive species induces rapid adaptive change in a native predator: cane toads and black snakes in Australia. Proc Royal Soc B Biol Sci 273(1593):1545

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips BL, Brown GP, Shine R (2003) Assessing the potential impact of cane toads on Australian snakes. Conserv Biol 17(6):1738–1747

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayward A (1974) Giant cane toads: a threat to Australian wildlife. Wildlife 17:506–507

    Google Scholar 

  • Rollo CD (2004) Life epigenetics, ecology, and evolution (L = E3): a review of developmental plasticity and evolution, by Mary Jane West-Eberhard. Evolut Dev 6(1):58–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer ML (1981) Minimum population sizes for species conservation. BioScience 31(2):131–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth SJ (2010) Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford University Press, USA

    Google Scholar 

  • Shine R (2010) The ecological impact of invasive cane toads (Bufo marinus) in Australia. Q Rev Biol 85(3):253–291

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Somaweera R, Webb JK, Brown GP, Shine RP (2011) Hatchling Australian freshwater crocodiles rapidly learn to avoid toxic invasive cane toads. Behaviour 148(4):501–517

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2003) Thought in a hostile world: the evolution of human cognition. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherst RW, Floyd RB, Maywald GF (1995) The potential geographical distribution of the cane toad, Bufo marinus, in Australia. Conserv Biol 10:294–299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tempel BL, Bonini N, Dawson DR, Quinn WG (1983) Reward learning in normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 80(5):1482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teotónio H, Chelo IM, Bradic M, Rose MR, Long AD (2009) Experimental evolution reveals natural selection on standing genetic variation. Nat Genet 41(2):251–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton A, McAuliffe K (2006) Teaching in wild meerkats. Science 313(5784):227–229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1994) The question of chimpanzee culture. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 301–317

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2009) The question of chimpanzee culture, plus postscript. In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 198–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Via S, Gomulkiewicz R, De Jong G, Scheiner SM, Schlichting CD, Van Tienderen PH (1995) Adaptive phenotypic plasticity: consensus and controversy. Trends Ecol Evol 10(5):212–217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner A (2005) Robustness, evolvability, and neutrality. FEBS Lett 579(8):1772–1778

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner A (2008) Robustness and evolvability: a paradox resolved. Proc Royal Soc B Biol Sci 275(1630):91–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb JK, Brown GP, Child T, Greenlees MJ, Phillips BL, Shine R (2008) A native dasyurid predator (common planigale, Planigale maculata) rapidly learns to avoid a toxic invader. Austral Ecol 33(7):821–829

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press, USA

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt WC (2007) Re-engineering philosophy for limited beings: piecewise approximations to reality. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am particularly grateful to Kevin Laland for his detailed comments and encouragement on the material in this paper. I would also like to acknowledge Brett Calcott, Ellen Clarke and Kim Sterelny along with audiences at the Australian National University and the 2011 Philosophy of Biology at Dolphin Beach Workshop in Moruya, NSW for their comments on earlier iterations the work. Funding for this research was provided through the generous support of an Australian National University PhD scholarship and a writing-up fellowship at the Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research in Altenberg, Austria.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rachael L. Brown.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brown, R.L. Learning, evolvability and exploratory behaviour: extending the evolutionary reach of learning. Biol Philos 28, 933–955 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-013-9396-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-013-9396-9

Keywords

Navigation