Skip to main content
Log in

Genetic Covariation Between Brain Volumes and IQ, Reading Performance, and Processing Speed

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although there has been much interest in the relation between brain size and cognition, few studies have investigated this relation within a genetic framework and fewer still in non-adult samples. We analyzed the genetic and environmental covariance between structural MRI data from four brain regions (total brain volume, neocortex, white matter, and prefrontal cortex), and four cognitive measures (verbal IQ (VIQ), performance IQ (PIQ), reading ability, and processing speed), in a sample of 41 MZ twin pairs and 30 same-sex DZ twin pairs (mean age at cognitive test = 11.4 years; mean age at scan = 15.4 years). Multivariate Cholesky decompositions were performed with each brain volume measure entered first, followed by the four cognitive measures. Consistent with previous research, each brain and cognitive measure was found to be significantly heritable. The novel finding was the significant genetic but not environmental covariance between brain volumes and cognitive measures. Specifically, PIQ shared significant common genetic variance with all four measures of brain volume (r g = .58–.82). In contrast, VIQ shared significant genetic influence with neocortex volume only (r g = .58). Processing speed was significant with total brain volume (r g = .79), neocortex (r g = .64), and white matter (r g = .89), but not prefrontal cortex. The only brain measure to share genetic influence with reading was total brain volume (r g = .32), which also shared genetic influences with processing speed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Cholesky decompositions were also done with each cognitive variable entered in the first position, and the four brain volumes entered as the second through fifth variables, and the pattern of results was the same as what is presented here.

References

  • Anstey K, Mack H, Christensen H, Li S, Reglade-Meslin C, Maller J et al (2007) Corpus callosum size, reaction time speed and variability in mild cognitive disorders and in a normative sample. Neuropsychologia 45:1911–1920

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baare WFC, Pol HE, Boomsma DI, Posthuma D, de Geus EJC, Schnack HG et al (2001) Quantitative genetic modeling of variation in human brain morphology. Cereb Cortex 11:816–824

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badian NA (1993) Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing, and reading. Read Writ Interdiscip J 5:87–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker LA, Vernon PA, Ho H (1991) The genetic correlation between intelligence and speed of information processing. Behav Genet 21:351–367

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Betjemann RS, Willcutt EG, Olson RK, Keenan JM, DeFries JC, Wadsworth SJ (2008) Word reading and comprehension: stability, overlap and independence. Read Writ Interdiscip J 21:539–558

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunce D, Anstey K, Christensen H, Dear K, Wen W, Sachdev P (2007) White matter hyperintensities and within-person variability in community-dwelling adults aged 60–64 years. Neuropsychologia 45:2009–2015

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne B, Olson RK, Samuelsson S, Wadsworth S, Corley R, DeFries JC, Willcutt E (2006) Genetic and environmental influences on early literacy. J Res Reading 29(1):33–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts HW, Gillispie M, Leonard LB, Kail RV, Miller CA (2002) The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. J Learn Disabil 35:509–524

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caviness VS Jr, Meyer J, Makris N, Kennedy DN (1996) MRI based topographic parcellation of human neocortex: an anatomically specified method with estimate of reliability. J Cogn Neurosci 8:566–587

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton DL, Davis CJ, DeFries JC, Gayan J, Olson RK (2001) Genetic and environmental influences on reading and RAN: an overview of results from the Colorado twin study. In: Wolf M (ed) Conference proceedings of the dyslexia research foundation conference in extraordinary brain series: time, fluency, and developmental dyslexia. York Press, Baltimore, MD, pp 277–303

    Google Scholar 

  • De Moor MHM, Boomsma DI, Stubbe JH, Willemsen G, De Geus EJC (2008) Testing causality in the association between regular exercise and symptoms of anxiety and depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:897–905

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeFries JC (1985) Colorado reading project. In: Gray DB, Kavanagh JF (eds) Biobehavioral measures of dyslexia. York Press, Parkton, MD, pp 107–122

    Google Scholar 

  • DeFries JC, Baker LA (1983) Colorado family reading study: longitudinal analyses. Ann Dyslexia 33:153–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeFries JC, Singer SM, Foch TT, Lewitter FI (1978) Familial nature of reading disability. Br J Psychiatry 132:361–367

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeFries JC, Filipek PA, Fulker DW, Olson RK, Pennington BF, Smith SD (1997) Colorado learning disabilities research center. Learn Disabil 8:7–19

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla MB, Rudel R (1974) Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex 10:186–202

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan J, Seitz RJ, Kolodny J, Bor D, Herzog H, Ahmed A, Newell FN, Emslie H (2000) A neural basis for general intelligence. Science 289:457–460

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn LM, Markwardt FC (1970) Examiner’s manual: Peabody individual achievement test. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Derman D (1976) Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests, 1976. Education Testing Service, Princeton, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Filipek PA, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Rossnick SL, Spraggins TA, Starewicz PM (1989) MRI-based morphometry: development and application to normal subjects. Ann Neurol 25:61–67

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Filley CM (2005) White matter and behavioral neurology. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1064:162–183

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher SE, DeFries JC (2002) Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:767–780

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fry AE, Hale S (1996) Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychol Sci 7:237–241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gayán J, Olson RK (2003) Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed word recognition. J Exp Child Psychol 84:97–123

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geidd JN, Schmitt JE, Neale MC (2007) Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of pediatric twins. Hum Brain Mapp 28:474–481

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ (1981) The mismeasure of man. WW Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho H-Z, Baker LA, Decker SN (1988) Covariation between intelligence and speed of cognitive processing: genetic and environmental influences. Behav Genet 18:247–261

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jerison HJ (1989) Brain size and the evolution of the mind. American Museum of Natural History, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung RE, Haier RJ (2007) The parieto-frontal integration theory of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav Brain Sci

  • Kail R (1991) Development of processing speed in childhood and adolescence. Adv Child Dev Behav 23:151–185

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy DN, Filipek PA, Caviness VS Jr (1989) Anatomic segmentation and volumetric calculations in nuclear magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 8:1–7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy DN, Lange N, Makirs N, Bates J, Meyer J, Caviness VS Jr (1998) Gyri of the human neocortex: an MRI-based analysis of volume and variance. Cereb Cortex 8:372–384

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenroot RK, Giedd JN (2007) The structural development of the human brain as measured longitudinally. In: Coch D, Fischer KF, Dawson G (eds) Human behavior, learning and the developing brain. The Guilford Press, New York, pp 50–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Luciano M, Smith GA, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Martin NG (2001a) On the heritability of inspection time and its covariance with IQ: a twin study. Intelligence 29:443–457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luciano M, Wright MJ, Smith GA, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Martin NG (2001b) Genetic covariance among measures of information processing speed, working memory, and IQ. Behav Genet 31:581–592

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luciano M, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Smith GA, Martin NG (2004) A genetic investigation of the covariation among inspection time, choice reaction time, and IQ subtest scores. Behav Genet 34:41–50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald S, Nyberg L, Bäckman L (2006) Intra-individual variability in behavior: links to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity. Trends Neurosci 29:474–480

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manis FR, Doi LM, Bhadha B (2000) Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. J Learn Disabil 33: 325–333, 374

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel MA (2005) Big-brained people are smarter: a meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intelligence 33:337–346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH (2002) Mx: statistical modeling, 6th edn. Department of Psychiatry, Richmond, VA

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ (1990) Automaticity: a new framework for dyslexia research. Cognition 35:159–182

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson RK (2004) SSSR, environment, and genes. Sci Stud Read 8:111–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington BF, Olson RK (2005) Genetics of dyslexia. In: Snowling M, Hulme C (eds) The science of reading: a handbook. Blackwell, Oxford, UK, pp 453–472

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington BF, Filipek PA, Lefly D, Churchwell J, Kennedy DN, Simon JH et al (1999) Brain morphometry in reading-disabled twins. Neurology 53:723–729

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington BF, Filipek PA, Lefly D, Chhabildas N, Kennedy DA, Simon JH et al (2000) A twin MRI study of size variations in the human brain. J Cogn Neurosci 12:223–232

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peper JS, Brouwer RM, Boomsma DI, Kahn RS, Pol HEH (2007) Genetic influences on human brain structure: a review of brain imaging studies in twins. Hum Brain Mapp 28:464–473

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petrill SA, Deater-Deckard K, Thompson LA, DeThorne L, Schatschneider C (2006) Reading skills in early readers: genetic and shared environmental influences. J Learn Disabil 39(1):48–55

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phinney E, Pennington BF, Olson RK, Filley CM, Filipek PA (2007) Brain structure correlates of component reading processes: implications for reading disability. Cortex 43:777–791

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P (2008) Behavioral genetics, 5th edn. Worth Publishers, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Posthuma D, de Geus EJC, Boomsma DI (2001) Perceptual speed and IQ are associated through common genetic factors. Behav Genet 31:593–602

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posthuma D, de Geus EJC, Baare WFC, Pol HEH, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI (2002) The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin. Nat Neurosci 5:83–84

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posthuma D, Baare WFC, Pol HEH, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI, de Gues EJC (2003) Brain volumes and the WAIS-III dimensions of verbal comprehension, working memory, perceptual organization, and processing speed. Twin Res 6:131–139

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelsson S, Byrne B, Quain P, Wadsworth S, Corley R, DeFries JC, Willcutt E, Olson RK (2005) Environmental and genetic influences on preschool skills in Australia, Scandinavia, and the United States. J Educ Psychol 97:705–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan MA, Pennington BF, Yerys BE, Scott A, Boada R, Willcutt EG, Olson RK, DeFries JC (2006) Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. J Abnorm Child Psychol 34:584–601

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan MA, McGrath L, Santerre-Lemmon L, Barnard H, Willcutt EG, Olson RK, Pennington BF (2007) Shared cognitive deficits in reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA

  • Thompson PM, Cannon TD, Narr KL, van Erp T, Poutanen V-P, Huttunen M et al (2001) Genetic influences on brain structure. Nat Neurosci 4:1253–1258

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen M, Peper JS, van den Berg SM, Brouwer RM, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI (2009) A genetic analysis of brain volumes and IQ in children. Intelligence 37:181–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler D (1974) Examiners’ manual: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children, 4th edn. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler D (2003) WISC-IV technical and interpretive manual. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX

    Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, Chhibildas N, Hulslander J (2005) Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of the common deficit. Dev Neuropsychol 27:35–78

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M, Bowers G (1999) The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. J Educ Psychol 91:415–438

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Initial work on this paper was done when R. Betjemann was a postdoctoral trainee at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO. This training was funded by NIMH training grant T32 MH016880-25. This project was also funded by NIH grant HD027802 to the Colorado Learning Disabilities Research Center, of which B. Pennington, J. DeFries, and E. Willcutt are Co-PIs.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca S. Betjemann.

Additional information

Edited by William Kremen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Betjemann, R.S., Johnson, E.P., Barnard, H. et al. Genetic Covariation Between Brain Volumes and IQ, Reading Performance, and Processing Speed. Behav Genet 40, 135–145 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-009-9328-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-009-9328-2

Keywords

Navigation