Advertisement

Archives of Sexual Behavior

, Volume 47, Issue 3, pp 661–668 | Cite as

Influence of Body Odors and Gender on Perceived Genital Arousal

  • Patrícia Alves-Oliveira
  • Joana Carvalho
  • Jacqueline Ferreira
  • Laura Alho
  • Pedro Nobre
  • Mats J. Olsson
  • Sandra C. Soares
Original Paper

Abstract

Olfaction is often linked to mating behavior in nonhumans. Additionally, studies in mating behavior have shown that women seem to be more affected by odor cues than men. However, the relationship between odor cues and sexual response—specifically, sexual arousal—has not been studied yet. The aim of this study was to evaluate the impact of the exposure to human body odors (from individuals of the opposite gender) on perceived genital arousal, while these were presented concomitantly to sexually explicit video clips. Eighty university students (40 women) rated their perceived genital arousal (perceived degree of erection/genital lubrication) in response to an audiovisual sexual stimulus, while simultaneously exposed to a body odor from an opposite-gender donor or no odor. Participants also rated each odor sample’s (body odor and no odor) perceived pleasantness, intensity, and familiarity. Findings indicated that odor condition had an effect on women’s (but not men’s) perceived genital arousal, with women showing higher levels of perceived genital arousal in the no odor condition. Also, results showed that women rated body odors as less pleasant than no odor. Notwithstanding, the odor ratings do not seem to explain the association between body odor and perceived genital arousal. The current results support the hypothesis that women, rather than men, are sensitive to odors in the context of sexual response. The findings of this study have relevance for the understanding of human sexuality with respect to chemosensory communication.

Keywords

Olfaction Human body odors Gender Perceived genital arousal Mating behavior 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by national funds through Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT). P. Alves-Oliveira acknowledges a FCT Grant Ref. SFRH/BD/110223/2015. This study was also supported by the Swedish Research Council (Grant 421-2012-1125) and The Swedish Foundation of Social Science and Humanities (Grant P12-1017) to M. J. Olsson. The authors show their gratitude to Elisa Pinto, Liliana Perdigão and Marta Rocha for their support during the body odor collection, to Dr. Pedro Bem-Haja for the data analysis support, and to all the participants for their involvement in the study.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

References

  1. Bendas, J. (2016). Odor threshold relates to sexual pleasure. Poster presented at the meeting of European Chemoreception Research Organization, Athens, Greece.Google Scholar
  2. Berglund, H., Lindström, P., & Savic, I. (2006). Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(21), 8269–8274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bossio, J. A., Suschinsky, K. D., Puts, D. A., & Chivers, M. L. (2014). Does menstrual cycle phase influence the gender specificity of heterosexual women’s genital and subjective sexual arousal? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(5), 941–952.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Carvalho, J., Gomes, A. Q., Laja, P., Oliveira, C., Vilarinho, S., Janssen, E., & Nobre, P. (2013). Gender differences in sexual arousal and affective responses to erotica: The effects of type of film and fantasy instructions. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(6), 1011–1019.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Cenfetelli, R., & Bassellier, G. (2009). Interpretation of formative measurement in information systems research. MIS Quarterly, 33(4), 689–708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chrea, C., Valentin, D., Sulmont-Rossé, C., Mai, H. L., Nguyen, D. H., & Abdi, H. (2004). Culture and odor categorization: Agreement between cultures depends upon the odors. Food Quality and Preference, 15(7), 669–679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Croy, I., Negoias, S., Novakova, L., Landis, B. N., & Hummel, T. (2012). Learning about the functions of the olfactory system from people without a sense of smell. PLoS ONE, 7(3), e33365–e33365.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Distel, H., Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Martínez-Gomez, M., Schicker, I., Kobayakawa, T., Saito, S., & Hudson, R. (1999). Perception of everyday odors—Correlation between intensity, familiarity and strength of hedonic judgement. Chemical Senses, 24(2), 191–199.  https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/24.2.191.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Distel, H., & Hudson, R. (2001). Judgement of odor intensity is influenced by subjects’ knowledge of the odor source. Chemical Senses, 26(3), 247–251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Doty, R. L. (1975). An examination of relationships between the pleasantness, intensity, and concentration of 10 odorous stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 17, 492–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Doty, R. L., & Cameron, E. L. (2009). Sex differences and reproductive hormone influences on human odor perception. Physiology & Behavior, 97(2), 213–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Franzoi, S. L., & Herzog, M. E. (1987). Judging physical attractiveness what body aspects do we use? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13(1), 19–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gower, D. B., Holland, K. T., Mallet, A. I., Rennie, P. J., & Watkins, W. J. (1994). Comparison of 16-androstene steroid concentrations in sterile apocrine sweat and axillary secretions: Interconversions of 16-androstenes by the axillary microflora—a mechanism for axillary odour production in man? Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 48(4), 409–418.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Graham, C. A., Janssen, E., & Sanders, S. A. (2000). Effects of fragrance on female sexual arousal and mood across the menstrual cycle. Psychophysiology, 37(1), 76–84.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Graham, C. A., Sanders, S. A., Milhausen, R. R., & McBride, K. R. (2004). Turning on and turning off: A focus group study of the factors that affect women’s sexual arousal. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(6), 527–538.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Havlicek, J., & Lenochova, P. (2006). The effect of meat consumption on body odor attractiveness. Chemical Senses, 31, 747–752.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Havlicek, J., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(4), 497–512.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Havlicek, J., Saxton, T. K., Roberts, S. C., Jozifkova, E., Lhota, S., Valentova, J., & Flegr, J. (2008). He sees, she smells? Male and female reports of sensory reliance in mate choice and non-mate choice contexts. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(6), 565–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Heckmann, M., Teichmann, B., Pause, B. M., & Plewig, G. (2003). Amelioration of body odor after intracutaneous axillary injection of botulinum toxin A. Archives of Dermatology, 139, 57–59.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Herz, R. S. (2004). A naturalistic analysis of autobiographical memories triggered by olfactory visual and auditory stimuli. Chemical Senses, 29(3), 217–224.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Herz, R. S., & Cahill, E. D. (1997). Differential use of sensory information in sexual behavior as a function of gender. Human Nature, 8(3), 275–286.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Herz, R. S., & Inzlicht, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to physical and social factors involved in human mate selection: The importance of smell for women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(5), 359–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jacob, S., McClintock, M. K., Zelano, B., & Ober, C. (2002). Paternally inherited HLA alleles are associated with women’s choice of male odor. Nature Genetics, 30(2), 175–179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., & Smith, M. J. L. (2008). Effects of menstrual cycle phase on face preferences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(1), 78–84.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Keller, A., & Vosshall, L. B. (2016). Olfactory perception of chemically diverse molecules. BMC Neuroscience, 17, 55.  https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-016-0287-2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Knaapila, A., Tuorila, H., Vuoksimaa, E., Keskitalo-Vuokko, K., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., & Silventoinen, K. (2012). Pleasantness of the odor of androstenone as a function of sexual intercourse experience in women and men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(6), 1403–1408.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Kromer, J., Hummel, T., Pietrowski, D., Giani, A. S., Sauter, J., Ehninger, G., … Croy, I. (2016). Influence of HLA on human partnership and sexual satisfaction. Scientific Reports, 6, 32550.  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep32550.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Lenochová, P., Roberts, S. C., & Havlicek, J. (2009). Methods of human body odor sampling: The effect of freezing. Chemical Senses, 34(2), 127–138.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Lenochová, P., Vohnoutova, P., Roberts, S. C., Oberzaucher, E., Grammer, K., & Havlíček, J. (2012). Psychology of fragrance use: Perception of individual odor and perfume blends reveals a mechanism for idiosyncratic effects on fragrance choice. PLoS ONE, 7(3), e33810.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033810.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Levin, R. (2004). Smells and tastes—their putative influence on sexual activity in humans. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 19(4), 451–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lübke, K. T., & Pause, B. M. (2015). Always follow your nose: The functional significance of social chemosignals in human reproduction and survival. Hormones and Behavior, 68, 134–144.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Martins, Y., Preti, G., Crabtree, C. R., Runyan, T., Vainius, A. A., & Wysocki, C. J. (2005). Preference for human body odors is influenced by gender and sexual orientation. Psychological Science, 16(9), 694–701.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. O’Brien, R. M. (2007). A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Quality & Quantity, 41, 673–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Penn, D., & Potts, W. (1998). How do major histocompatibility complex genes influence odor and mating preferences. Advances in Immunology, 69, 411–436.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Roberts, S. C., Gosling, L. M., Carter, V., & Petrie, M. (2008). MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 275(1652), 2715–2722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Savic, I., Berglund, H., & Lindström, P. (2005). Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(20), 7356–7361.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Semin, G. R., & De Groot, J. H. (2013). The chemical bases of human sociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 427–429.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Sodavari, M., Shahidi, M. A., Almadani, S. A. H., Moazedian, A., & Imani, M. S. (2014). The role of pleasant and unpleasant odors in the individual and social attractiveness. Journal of Educational and Management Studies, 4(2), 311–315.Google Scholar
  39. Sorokowska, A., Butovskaya, M., & Veselovskaya, E. (2015). Partner’s body odor versus relatives’ body odor: A comparison of female associations. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 46(2), 209–213.Google Scholar
  40. Stevenson, R. J. (2010). An initial evaluation of the functions of human olfaction. Chemical Senses, 35(1), 3–20.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Suschinsky, K. D., & Lalumiere, M. L. (2012). Is sexual concordance related to awareness of physiological states? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(1), 199–208.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Wedekind, C., & Füri, S. (1997). Body odour preferences in men and women: Do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1387), 1471–1479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Wedekind, C., Seebeck, T., Bettens, F., & Paepke, A. J. (1995). MHC-dependent mate preferences in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 260(1359), 245–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wyatt, T. D. (2003). Pheromones and animal behaviour: Communication by smell and taste. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE-IUL)CIS-IULLisbonPortugal
  2. 2.Escola de Psicologia e Ciências da Vida Universidade Lusófona de Humanidades e TecnologiasLisbonPortugal
  3. 3.Centro de Psicologia da Universidade do PortoPortoPortugal
  4. 4.Department of Education and PsychologyUniversity of AveiroAveiroPortugal
  5. 5.Center for Health Technology and Services Research, Faculty of MedicineUniversity of PortoPortoPortugal
  6. 6.Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Life Sciences, Faculty of MedicineUniversity of CoimbraCoimbraPortugal
  7. 7.Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da EducaçãoUniversidade do PortoPortoPortugal
  8. 8.Division of Psychology, Department of Clinical NeuroscienceKarolinska InstituteSolnaSweden
  9. 9.Instituto de Engenharia de Sistemas e Computadores - Investigação e DesenvolvimentoPorto SalvoPortugal
  10. 10.William James Center for ResearchISPA University InstituteLisbonPortugal

Personalised recommendations