Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

When Parsimony Is Not Enough: Considering Dual Processes and Dual Levels of Influence in Sexual Decision Making

  • Special Section: Sexual Health in Gay and Bisexual Men
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The literature on sexual decision making that has been used to understand behaviors relevant to HIV and STI risk has relied primarily on cognitive antecedents of behavior. In contrast, several prominent models of decision making outside of the sexual behavior literature rely on dual process models, in which both affective and cognitive processing are considered as important precursors to behavior. Moreover, much of the literature on sexual behavior utilizes individual-level traits and characteristics to predict aggregated sexual behavior, despite decision making itself being a situational or event-level process. This article proposes a framework for understanding sexual decision making as the result of dual processes (affective and cognitive) operating at dual level of influence (individual and situational). Finally, this article ends with a discussion of the conceptual and methodological benefits and challenges to its use and future directions for research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Millsap, R. E. (2008). Doctoral training in statistics, measurement, and methodology in psychology: Replication and extension of Aiken, West, Sechrest, and Reno’s (1990) survey of PhD programs in North America. American Psychologist, 63, 32–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albarracín, D., Durantini, M. R., & Earl, A. (2006). Empirical and theoretical conclusions of an analysis of outcomes of HIV-prevention interventions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 73–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albarracín, D., Gillette, J. C., Earl, A. N., Glasman, L. R., Durantini, M. R., & Ho, M. H. (2005). A test of major assumptions about behavior change: A comprehensive look at the effects of passive and active HIV-prevention interventions since the beginning of the epidemic. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 856–897.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., Fishbein, M., & Muellerleile, P. A. (2001). Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 142–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. F. (2006). The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual arousal on sexual decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 87–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1976). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development. Six theories of child development (Vol. 6, pp. 1–60). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P. (2011). Dual-process theories and cognitive development: Advances and challenges. Developmental Review, 31, 79–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateman, I., Dent, S., Peters, E., Slovic, P., & Starmer, C. (2007). The affect heuristic and the attractiveness of simple gambles. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20, 365–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Engel, C. L., & Fridell, S. R. (1999). Value judgments: Testing the somatic-marker hypothesis using false physiological feedback. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1021–1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., & Lee, G. P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5473–5481.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Dolan, S., Denburg, N., Hindes, A., Anderson, S. W., & Nathan, P. E. (2001). Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia, 39, 376–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanton, H., & Gerrard, M. (1997). Effect of sexual motivation on men’s risk perception for sexually transmitted disease: There must be 50 ways to justify a lover. Health Psychology, 16, 374–379. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.16.4.374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boldero, J., Moore, S., & Rosenthal, D. (1992). Intention, context, and safe sex: Australian adolescents’ responses to AIDS. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 1374–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods: An introduction to diary and experience sampling research. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burleson, M. H., Trevathan, W. R., & Todd, M. (2007). In the mood for love or vice versa? Exploring the relations among sexual activity, physical affection, affect, and stress in the daily lives of mid-aged women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 357–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, S., & Pasqualini, M. C. S. (2004). Stronger autonomic response accompanies better learning: A test of Damasio’s somatic marker hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 901–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Estimated HIV incidence among adults and adolescents in the United States, 2007–2010. HIV Surveillance Supplemental Report, 17(4). Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/surveillance/resources/reports/2010supp_vol17no4/pdf/hssr_vol_17_no_4.pdf#page=14.

  • Cooper, M. L. (2010). Toward a person × situation model of sexual risk-taking behaviors: Illuminating the conditional effects of traits across sexual situations and relationship contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 319–341. doi:10.1037/a0017785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crabtree, M. A. (2012). The role of rejection in women’s sexual decision making: An experimental vignette study. Master’s thesis, University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio.

  • Crepaz, N., & Marks, G. (2001). Are negative affective states associated with HIV sexual risk behaviors? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 20, 291–299. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.20.4.291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Penguin Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (2001). Emotion and the human brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 935, 101–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ditto, P. H., Pizarro, D. A., Epstein, E. B., Jacobson, J. A., & MacDonald, T. K. (2006). Visceral influences on risk-taking behavior. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 99–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durham, M. D., Buchacz, K., Richardson, J., Yang, D., Wood, K., Yangco, B., & Brooks, J. T. (2013). Sexual risk behavior and viremia among men who have sex with men in the HIV Outpatient Study, United States, 2007–2010. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 63, 372–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M. (1990). AIDS and behavior change: An analysis based on the theory of reasoned action. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia, 24, 37–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Middlestadt, S. E. (1989). Using the theory of reasoned action as a framework for understanding and changing AIDS-related behaviors. In V. M. Mays, G. W. Albee, & S. F. Schneider (Eds.), Primary prevention of AIDS: Psychological approaches (pp. 93–110). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., & Fisher, W. A. (1992). Changing AIDS-risk behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 455–474. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.3.455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., & Fisher, W. A. (2000). Theoretical approaches to individual-level change in HIV risk behavior. In J. L. Peterson & R. J. DiClemente (Eds.), Handbook of HIV prevention (pp. 3–55). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., Fisher, W. A., Misovich, S. J., Kimble, D. L., & Malloy, T. E. (1996). Changing AIDS risk behavior: Effects of an intervention emphasizing AIDS risk reduction information, motivation, and behavioral skills in a college student population. Health Psychology, 15, 114–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., Fisher, W. A., & Shuper, P. A. (2009). The information-motivation-behavioral skills model of HIV preventive behavior. In R. J. DiClemente, R. A. Crosby, & M. C. Kegler (Eds.), Emerging theories in health promotion practice and research (2nd ed., pp. 21–63). San Francisco: Jossy-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., Fisher, W. A., Williams, S. S., & Malloy, T. E. (1994). Empirical tests of an information-motivation-behavioral skills model of AIDS-preventive behavior with gay men and heterosexual university students. Health Psychology, 13, 238–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, W. A., Williams, S. S., Fisher, J. D., & Malloy, T. E. (1999). Understanding AIDS risk behavior among sexually active urban adolescents: An empirical test of the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. AIDS and Behavior, 3, 13–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortenberry, J. D., Temkit, M. H., Tu, W., Graham, C. A., Katz, B. P., & Orr, D. P. (2005). Daily mood, partner support, sexual interest, and sexual activity among adolescent women. Health Psychology, 24, 252–257. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.24.3.252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frankish, K. (2010). Dual-process and dual-system theories of reasoning. Philosophy Compass, 5, 914–926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golub, S. A., Starks, T. J., Payton, G., & Parsons, J. T. (2011). The critical role of intimacy in the sexual risk behaviors of gay and bisexual men. AIDS and Behavior, 16, 626–632. doi:10.1007/s10461-011-9972-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grov, C., Golub, S. A., Mustanski, B., & Parsons, J. T. (2010). Sexual compulsivity, state affect, and sexual risk behavior in a daily diary study of gay and bisexual men. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24, 487–497. doi:10.1037/a0020527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harless, D. W., & Camerer, C. F. (1994). The predictive utility of generalized expected utility theories. Econometrica Journal of the Econometric Society, 62, 1251–1289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, J. A., Mullen, P. D., & Green, L. W. (1992). A meta-analysis of studies of the health belief model with adults. Health Education Research, 7, 107–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. C., & Nutt, P. C. (1980). The influence of decision style on decision making behavior. Management Science, 26, 371–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hine, D. W., Marks, A. D., Nachreiner, M., Gifford, R., & Heath, Y. (2007). Keeping the home fires burning: The affect heuristic and wood smoke pollution. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27, 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirshleifer, D., & Shumway, T. (2003). Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather. Journal of Finance, 58, 1009–1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huebner, D. M., Neilands, T. B., Rebchook, G. M., & Kegeles, S. M. (2011). Sorting through chickens and eggs: A longitudinal examination of the associations between attitudes, norms, and sexual risk behavior. Health Psychology, 30, 110–118. doi:10.1037/a0021973.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, R. G., Krzystofiak, F. J., Meindl, J. R., & Yousry, A. M. (1989). Cognitive style and decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44, 436–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884). What is emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janz, N. K., Champion, V. L., & Strecher, V. J. (2002). The health belief model. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer, & F. M. Lewis (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice (3rd ed., pp. 45–66). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, W. D., Hedges, L. V., Ramirez, G., Semaan, S., Norman, L. R., Sogolow, E., & Diaz, R. M. (2002). HIV prevention research for men who have sex with men: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 30, S118–S129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalichman, S. C., & Weinhardt, L. (2001). Negative affect and sexual risk behavior: Comment on Crepaz and Marks (2001). Health Psychology, 20, 300–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster Paperbacks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R. (1994). “Hot” versus “cold” cognition: An Abelsonian voyage. In R. C. Schank & E. Langer (Eds.), Beliefs, reasoning, and decision making: Psycho-logic in honor of Bob Abelson (pp. 237–251). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 473–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Nagin, D., & Paternoster, R. (1997). The effect of sexual arousal on expectations of sexual forcefulness. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 34, 443–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, M. P. (2005). The role of performance anxiety in the development and maintenance of sexual dysfunction in men and women. International Journal of Stress Management, 12, 379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehl, M. R., & Conner, T. S. (2012). Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mongin, P. (1998). Expected utility theory. In J. B. Davis, D. W. Hands, & U. Maki (Eds.), Handbook of economic methodology (pp. 342–350). London: Edward Elgar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monroe, K. R., & Maher, K. H. (1995). Psychology and rational actor theory. Political Psychology, 16, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montaño, D. E., & Kasprzyk, D. (2002). The theory of reasoned action and the theory of planned behavior. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer, & F. M. Lewis (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice (3rd ed., pp. 99–120). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mustanski, B. (2004). The relationship between mood and sexual interest, behavior, and risk-taking. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN.

  • Mustanski, B. (2007). The influence of state and trait affect on HIV risk behaviors: A daily diary study of MSM. Health Psychology, 26, 618–626. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.26.5.618.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, I. B. (1987). Introduction to type: A description of the theory and applications of the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noar, S. M. (2007). An interventionist’s guide to AIDS behavioral theories. AIDS Care, 19, 392–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noar, S. M. (2008). Behavioral interventions to reduce HIV-related sexual risk behavior: Review and synthesis of meta-analytic evidence. AIDS and Behavior, 12, 335–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, S. D., Pazienza, N. J., & Ferrin, H. H. (1984). Decision-making styles and problem-solving appraisal. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 497–502. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redlawsk, D. P. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics, 64, 1021–1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reece, M., Herbenick, D., Schick, V., Sanders, S. A., Dodge, B., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2010). Condom use rates in a national probability sample of males and females ages 14 to 94 in the United States. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 7, 266–276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riker, W. H. (1995). The political psychology of rational choice theory. Political Psychology, 16, 23–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, W. S. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals. American Sociological Review, 15, 351–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Röcke, C., Li, S.-C., & Smith, J. (2009). Intraindividual variability in positive and negative affect over 45 days: Do older adults fluctuate less than young adults? Psychology and Aging, 24, 863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberger, J. G., Reece, M., Schick, V., Herbenick, D., Novak, D. S., Van Der Pol, B., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2012). Condom use during most recent anal intercourse event among a US sample of men who have sex with men. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9, 1037–1047.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenstreich, Y., & Hsee, C. K. (2001). Money, kisses, and electric shocks: On the affective psychology of risk. Psychological Science, 12, 185–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. J., Moskowitz, D., Zuroff, D. C., Sookman, D., & Paris, J. (2007). Stability and variability of affective experience and interpersonal behavior in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, S. A., Reece, M., Herbenick, D., Schick, V., Dodge, B., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2010). Condom use during most recent vaginal intercourse event among a probability sample of adults in the United States. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 7, 362–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1998). Do intentions predict condom use? Meta-analysis and examination of six moderator variables. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 231–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., & Taylor, S. (1999). Predicting intentions to use condoms: A meta-analysis and comparison of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 1624–1675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrier, L. A., Shih, M. C., & Beardslee, W. R. (2005). Affect and sexual behavior in adolescents: A review of the literature and comparison of momentary sampling with diary and retrospective self-report methods of measurement. Pediatrics, 115, e573–e581.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24, 311–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Operational Research, 177, 1333–1352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, A., Hirota, A., Takasawa, N., & Shigemasu, K. (2003). Application of the somatic marker hypothesis to individual differences in decision making. Biological Psychology, 65, 81–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teitelman, A. M., Tennille, J., Bohinski, J. M., Jemmott, L. S., & Jemmott, J. B. III. (2011). Unwanted unprotected sex: Condom coercion by male partners and self-silencing of condom negotiation among adolescent girls. Advances in Nursing Science, 34, 243–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tyson, M., Covey, J., & Rosenthal, H. E. (2014). Theory of planned behavior interventions for reducing heterosexual risk behaviors: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 33, 1454–1467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, B. E., Golub, S. A., & Parsons, J. T. (2011). An integrated theoretical approach to substance use and risky sexual behavior among men who have sex with men. AIDS and Behavior, 15, 509–520.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.39.2.117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The development of this article was supported in part by an Individual Predoctoral Fellowship by the National Institute of Mental Health (F31-MH095622; Rendina: PI) and a Career Development Award from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (K01-DA039030; Rendina: PI). I would like to acknowledge the support of Dr. Jeffrey Parsons and Dr. Sarit Golub, both of whom provided considerable support and feedback on previous drafts. I would also like to thank Dr. Kristi Gamarel and Brett Millar for their feedback on this article, Dr. John Pachankis, Dr. Brian Mustanski, and Dr. Kristin Sommer for their feedback on previous drafts, and the attendees of the International Meeting on Contemporary Issues in Gay Men’s Sexual Health Research for thoughtful questions and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to H. Jonathon Rendina.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rendina, H.J. When Parsimony Is Not Enough: Considering Dual Processes and Dual Levels of Influence in Sexual Decision Making. Arch Sex Behav 44, 1937–1947 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0569-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0569-2

Keywords

Navigation