Abstract
Empirical research has documented that contact with lesbians and gays is associated with more positive feelings toward and greater support for legal rights for them, but we know less about whether these effects extend to informal aspects of same-sex relationships, such as reactions to public displays of affection. Furthermore, many studies have assumed that contact influences levels of sexual prejudice; however, the possibility of selection effects, in which less sexually prejudiced people have contact, and more sexually prejudiced people do not, raises some doubts about this assumption. We used original data from a nationally representative sample of heterosexuals to determine whether those reporting contact with a lesbian, gay, bisexual, or transgender friend or relative exhibited less sexual prejudice toward lesbian and gay couples than those without contact. This study examined the effect of contact on attitudes toward formal rights and a relatively unexplored dimension, informal privileges. We estimated the effect of having contact using traditional (ordinary least squares regression) methods before accounting for selection effects using propensity score matching. After accounting for selection effects, we found no significant differences between the attitudes of those who had contact and those who did not, for either formal or informal measures. Thus, selection effects appeared to play a pivotal role in confounding the link between contact and sexual prejudice, and future studies should exercise caution in interpreting results that do not account for such selection effects.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Alexander, L. A., & Link, B. G. (2003). The impact of contact on stigmatizing attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 271–289.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Altemeyer, B. (2001). Changes in attitudes toward homosexuals. Journal of Homosexuality, 42, 63–75.
Amir, Y. (1969). Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psychological Bulletin, 71, 319–342.
Basow, S. A., & Johnson, K. (2000). Predictors of homophobia in female college students. Sex Roles, 42, 391–404.
Binder, J., Brown, R., Funk, F., Kesller, T., Amelie Mummendey, A., & Zagefka, H. (2009). Does contact reduce prejudice or does prejudice reduce contact? A longitudinal test of the contact hypothesis among majority and minority groups in three European countries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 843–856.
Blalock, H. M. (1957). Percent non-white and discrimination in the South. American Sociological Review, 22, 677–682.
Bogardus, E. S. (1933). A social distance scale. Sociology & Social Research, 17, 265–271.
Britton, D. M. (1990). Homophobia and homosociality. Sociological Quarterly, 31, 423–439.
Brown, R. (2010). Prejudice: Its social psychology (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Brown, M. J., & Henriquez, E. (2011). Support for gay and lesbian civil rights: Development and examination of a new scale. Journal of Homosexuality, 58, 462–475.
Brunton, K. (1997). Stigma. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 26, 891–898.
Caliendo, M., & Kopeinig, S. (2005). Some practical guidance for the implementation of propensity score matching. IZA Discussion Papers, No. 1588, Institute for the Study of Labor.
Caspi, A. (1984). Contact hypothesis and inter-age attitudes: A field-study of cross-age contact. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47, 74–80.
Christ, O., & Wagner, U. (2012). Methodological issues in the study of intergroup contact: Towards a new wave of research. In G. Hodson & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Advances in intergroup contact (pp. 233–261). New York: Psychology Press.
Christafore, D., & Leguizamon, S. (2012). The influence of gay and lesbian coupled households on house prices in conservative and liberal neighborhoods. Journal of Urban Economics, 71, 258–267.
Cook, S. W. (1978). Interpersonal and attitudinal outcomes in cooperating interracial groups. Journal of Research & Development in Education, 12, 97–113.
Cotten-Huston, A. L., & Waite, B. M. (1999). Anti-homosexual attitudes in college students. Journal of Homosexuality, 38, 117–133.
Cunningham, G. B., & Melton, E. N. (2013). The moderating effects of contact with lesbian and gay friends on the relationships among religious fundamentalism, sexism, and sexual prejudice. Journal of Sex Research, 50, 401–408.
Deutsch, M., & Collins, M. E. (1951). Interracial housing: A psychological evaluation of a social experiment. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Doan, L., Loehr, A., & Miller, L. (2014). Formal rights and informal privileges for same-sex couples: Evidence from a national survey experiment. American Sociological Review, 79, 1172–1195.
Freese, J., & Visser, P. (2010). Data collected by time-sharing experiments for the social sciences. NSF Grant 0818839.
Gallup/Newsweek Poll. (1983, July 20–21). “National adult telephone survey.”
Gentry, C. S. (1986). Development of scales measuring social distance toward male and female homosexuals. Journal of Homosexuality, 13, 75–82.
Harder, V. S., Stuart, E. A., & Anthony, J. C. (2010). Propensity score techniques and the assessment of measured covariate balance to test causal associations in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 15, 234–249.
Herek, G. M. (1994). Assessing attitudes toward lesbians and gay men: A review of empirical research with the ATLG scale. In B. Greene & G. M. Herek (Eds.), Lesbian and gay psychology (pp. 206–228). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Herek, G. M. (2000). The psychology of sexual prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 19–22.
Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1996). “Some of my best friends”: Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 412–424.
Herek, G. M., & Glunt, E. K. (1993). Interpersonal contact and heterosexuals attitudes toward gay men: Results from a national survey. Journal of Sex Research, 30, 239–244.
Krahe, B., & Altwasser, C. (2006). Changing negative attitudes towards persons with physical disabilities: An experimental intervention. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 16, 59–69.
LaCour, M. J., & Green, D. P. (2014). When contact changes minds: An experiment on transmission of support for gay equality. Science, 346, 1366–1369.
Lance, L. M. (1987). The effects of interaction with gay persons on attitudes toward homosexuality. Human Relations, 40, 329–336.
Lemm, K. M. (2006). Positive associations among interpersonal contact, motivation, and implicit and explicit attitudes toward gay men. Journal of Homosexuality, 51, 79–99.
Levin, S., van Laar, C., & Sidanius, J. (2003). The effects of ingroup and outgroup friendships on ethnic attitudes in college: A longitudinal study. Group Processes Intergroup Relations, 6, 76–92.
Lewis, G. B. (2007). Personal relationships and support for gay rights (Working Paper No. 07-10). Retrieved from Social Science Research Network website: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=975975.
Lewis, G. B. (2011). The friends and family plan: Contact with gays and support for gay rights. Policy Studies Journal, 39, 217–238.
Lewis, G. B., & Gossett, C. W. (2008). Changing public opinion on same-sex marriage: The case of California. Politics & Policy, 36, 4–30.
Loftus, J. (2001). America’s liberalization in attitudes toward homosexuality, 1973 to 1998. American Sociological Review, 66, 762–782.
Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., Olafsdottir, S., & McLeod, J. D. (2007). The construction of fear: Americans’ preferences for social distance from children and adolescents with mental health problems. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 48, 50–67.
Massoglia, M. (2008). Incarceration as exposure: The prison, infectious disease, and other stress-related illnesses. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 49, 56–71.
McIntosh, P. (2003). White privilege and male privilege: A personal account of coming to see correspondences through work in women’s studies. In M. S. Kimmel & A. L. Ferber (Eds.), Privilege: A reader (pp. 147–160). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
McVeigh, R., & Diaz, M. D. (2009). Voting to ban same-sex marriage: Interests, values, and communities. American Sociological Review, 74, 891–915.
Miller, N. (2002). Personalization and the promise of contact theory. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 387–410.
Mohipp, C., & Morry, M. M. (2004). The relationship of symbolic beliefs and prior contact to heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbian women. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 36, 36–44.
Overby, L. M., & Barth, J. (2002). Contact, community context, and public attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Polity, 34, 433–456.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 65–85.
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751–783.
Powell, B., Bolzendahl, C., Geist, C., & Steelman, L. C. (2010). Counted out: Same-sex relations and Americans’ definitions of family. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Powers, D. A., & Ellison, C. G. (1995). Interracial contact and black racial-attitudes: The contact hypothesis and selectivity bias. Social Forces, 74, 205–226.
Rosenbaum, P. R. (2002). Observational studies (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.
Rosenbaum, P. R., & Rubin, D. B. (1983). The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika, 70, 41–55.
Rosenbaum, P. R., & Rubin, D. B. (1985). Constructing a control group using multivariate matched sampling methods that incorporate the propensity score. American Statistician, 39, 33–38.
Ruel, E., & Campbell, R. T. (2006). Homophobia and HIV/AIDS: Attitude change in the face of an epidemic. Social Forces, 84, 2167–2178.
Shadish, W. R. (2010). Campbell and Rubin: A primer and comparison of their approaches to causal inference in field settings. Psychological Methods, 15, 3–17.
Skipworth, S. A., Garner, A., & Dettrey, B. J. (2010). Limitations of the contact hypothesis: Heterogeneity in the contact effect on attitudes toward gay rights. Politics & Policy, 38, 887–906.
Smith, S. J., Axelton, A. M., & Saucier, D. A. (2009). The effects of contact on sexual prejudice: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 61, 178–191.
Smith-Lovin, L. (2007). The strength of weak identities: Social structural sources of self, situation and emotional experience. Social Psychology Quarterly, 70, 106–124.
South, S. J., Bonjean, C. M., Markham, W. T., & Corder, J. (1982). Social-structure and inter-group interaction: Men and women of the federal bureaucracy. American Sociological Review, 47, 587–599.
Van Dick, R., Wagner, U., Pettigrew, T. F., Christ, O., Wolf, C., Petzel, T., … Jackson, J. S. (2004). Role of perceived importance in intergroup contact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 211–227.
Wilder, D. A. (1984). Intergroup contact: The typical member and the exception to the rule. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 177–194.
Acknowledgments
We thank Brian Powell, Eliza Pavalko, Erick Janssen, the anonymous reviewers, and the Editor for insightful comments and excellent suggestions on various drafts of this article. This article was presented at Indiana University’s Gender/Race/Class workshop, the Sexual Science Research Seminar, and the Social Stratification and Individual Lives Seminar. We thank members of the workshop and seminar for their valuable comments and suggestions. We gratefully acknowledge support from the National Science Foundation through a Graduate Research Fellowship to the second author (NSF Grant DGE-0813962) and the Williams Institute’s Small Grants Program for this project. We thank Jack Martin for help with the Williams Institute grant. Data for this project were collected by Time-sharing Experiments for the Social Sciences (TESS), NSF Grant 0818839, and we thank the TESS PIs, Jeremy Freese and Penny Visser, and anonymous reviewers for their suggestions on our design. Opinions, findings, and conclusions presented in this article are ours and do not necessarily represent the views of any of these funding organizations.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendices
Appendix 1
Question Wording
Items were coded in the analyses so that 1 = Strongly disagree, 2 = Somewhat disagree, 3 = Somewhat agree, and 4 = Strongly agree. To facilitate participants’ understandings of the partnership benefits questions, contextualizing information introduced all of these items. Questions briefly explained that one of the couple members was seriously ill (family leave), had been hospitalized after a car accident (hospital visitation rights), did not have health insurance (health insurance benefits), or had died unexpectedly without living relatives or a last will and testament (inheritance rights).
Formal Rights
-
1.
Family leave.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that [the vignette characters] should be able to take time off from work without being paid to take care of the sick partner?
-
2.
Hospital visitation.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that [the vignette characters] should have hospital visitation rights that parents, children, and husbands and wives have?
-
3.
Insurance benefits.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that [the vignette characters] should have health insurance benefits for the partner?
-
4.
Inheritance rights.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that [the vignette characters] should have inheritance rights similar to a husband and wife?
Informal Privileges
-
1.
Tell others.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that it is okay for [the vignette characters] to tell others that they are a couple?
-
2.
Hold hands.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that it is okay for [the vignette characters] to hold each other’s hands in a park?
-
3.
Kiss on cheek.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that it is okay for [the vignette characters] to give each other a kiss on the cheek in a park?
-
4.
French kiss.
To what extent do you agree or disagree that it is okay for [the vignette characters] to French kiss in a park?
Appendix 2
Vignette Wording
Vignette character names were chosen to signal membership in similar birth cohorts to reduce the potentially confounding effects of perceived age differences between the partners. In the United States, “Brian,” “Jennifer,” “Matt,” and “Heather” were all within the top 20 most popular names for the late 1970s and early 1980s.
Instructions
We would like to know what you think about the following unmarried couple. Please take some time to form an impression about the couple once you have read their story. Next, please answer the questions that follow.
Vignette 1: The gay couple Brian and Matt met 3 years ago and were immediately attracted to each other. After going on a few dates, Brian told Matt that he wanted to see him exclusively, to which Matt happily agreed; they’ve been together ever since. Although Brian and Matt have had serious arguments, they both report being happy when they are together. Brian and Matt feel complete trust in each other. In fact, they’ve lived together for the past 2 years.
Vignette 2: The lesbian couple Heather and Jennifer met 3 years ago and were immediately attracted to each other. After going on a few dates, Heather told Jennifer that she wanted to see her exclusively, to which Jennifer happily agreed; they’ve been together ever since. Although Heather and Jennifer have had serious arguments, they both report being happy when they are together. Heather and Jennifer feel complete trust in each other. In fact, they’ve lived together for the past 2 years.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Loehr, A., Doan, L. & Miller, L.R. The Role of Selection Effects in the Contact Hypothesis: Results from a U.S. National Survey on Sexual Prejudice. Arch Sex Behav 44, 2111–2123 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0483-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0483-7