Skip to main content
Log in

Individual Differences in Perceptions of Gay Men’s Sexual Role Preferences from Facial Cues

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research has demonstrated that the sexual role preferences of gay men can be perceived with accuracies that exceed chance guessing from viewing photos of their faces. This research was conducted with only heterosexual perceivers making the categorizations. We therefore examined whether men who have sex with men (N = 121) were able to perceive sexual role preferences from faces and, critically, whether perceivers’ masculinity, femininity, homonegativity, and own sexual role preference affected their categorizations of targets as “tops” or “bottoms.” We found that men who have sex with men, like heterosexual perceivers in prior work, perceived gay men’s sexual role preferences accurately. Furthermore, men who self-identified with a receptive (bottom) role were more accurate in their categorizations and male perceivers who self-reported higher levels of masculinity were more likely to categorize other men as bottoms. These findings suggest that men’s masculinity could serve as a lens through which people perceive others and interact with the world.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Bisexual men did not differ from gay men on any measures of interest.

  2. The hit and the false alarm rates were used in the calculations of accuracy and bias. Thus, we do not focus on the separate effects for these variables in our discussion. However, more masculine people had both greater hit and false alarm rates, suggesting the possibility of a response bias.

  3. Because the sample of bisexual men was too small for any statistical inference, we examined gay men as a separate sample and found effects that were similar in magnitude and direction. All patterns of statistical significance were the same.

References

  • Andrzejewski, S. A., Hall, J. A., & Salib, E. R. (2009). Outgroup prejudice and accuracy of identifying outgroup membership: The case of anti-Semitism. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 47–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (1991). The influence of testosterone on human aggression. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 1–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, J. M., Kim, P. Y., Hills, A., & Linsenmeier, J. A. W. (1997). Butch, femme, or straight acting? Partner preferences of gay men and lesbians. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 960–973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1981). Bem sex-role inventory: Professional manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M. D., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carballo-Diéguez, A., Dolezal, C., Nieves, L., Diaz, F., Decena, C., & Balan, I. (2004). Looking for a tall, dark, macho man … Sexual-role behavior variations in Latino gay and bisexual men. Culture, Health, & Sexuality, 6, 159–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dabbs, J. M., & Mallinger, A. (1999). High testosterone levels predict low voice pitch among men. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 801–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Law Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., … Perrett, D. I. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 215–222.

  • Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., Karney, B. R., Reis, H. T., & Sprecher, S. (2012). Online dating: A critical analysis from the perspective of psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13, 3–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gil, S. (2007). A narrative exploration of gay men’s sexual practices as a dialectical dialogue. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 22, 63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Rosip, J. C., Smith LeBeau, L., Horgan, T. G., & Carter, J. D. (2006). Attributing the sources of accuracy in unequal-power dyadic communication: Who is better and why? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 18–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, C. S. (2008). A qualitative exploration of the relationship between racism and unsafe sex among Asian Pacific Islander gay men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 827–837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, T. A., Wolitski, R. J., Purcell, D. W., Gómez, C., & The Seropositive Urban Men’s Study Team. (2003). Sexual behavior among HIV-positive men who have sex with men: What’s in a label? Journal of Sex Research, 40, 179–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kippax, S., & Smith, G. (2001). Anal intercourse and power in sex between men. Sexualities, 4, 413–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., & Keltner, D. (2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 1716–1723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mignault, A., & Chaudhuri, A. (2003). The many faces of a neutral face: Head tilt and perception of dominance and emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 111–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, M. A., & Morrison, T. G. (2003). Development and validation of a scale measuring modern prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women. Journal of Homosexuality, 43, 15–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, D. A., & Hart, T. A. (2011). The influence of physical body traits and masculinity on anal sex roles in gay and bisexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 835–841.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, D. A., Rieger, G., & Roloff, M. E. (2008). Tops, bottoms and versatiles. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 23, 191–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, D. A., Rieger, G., & Seal, D. W. (2009). Narcissism, self-evaluation, and partner preferences among men who have sex with men. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 725–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pachankis, J. E., Buttenwieser, I. G., Bernstein, L. B., & Bayles, D. O. (2013). A longitudinal, mixed methods study of sexual position identity, behavior, and fantasies among young sexual minority men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1241–1253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penton-Voak, I. S., & Chen, J. Y. (2004). High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 229–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–887.

  • Raymond, H. F., & McFarland, W. (2009). An examination of the rates of receptive anal intercourse among Asian/Pacific Islander men who have sex with men in San Francisco. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 168–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rieger, G., Linsenmeier, J. A. W., Gygax, L., Garcia, S., & Bailey, J. M. (2010). Dissecting “gaydar”: Accuracy and the role of masculinity-femininity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 124–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (2008). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

  • Rule, N. O., Ishii, K., Ambady, N., Rosen, K. S., & Hallett, K. C. (2011). Found in translation: Cross-cultural consensus in the accurate categorization of male sexual orientation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1449–1507.

  • Tan, J. Y., Pratto, F., Operario, D., & Dworkin, S. L. (2013). Sexual positioning and race-based attraction by preferences for social dominance among gay Asian/Pacific Islander men in the United States. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1233–1239.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taywaditep, K. J. (2002). Marginalization among the marginalized: Gay men’s anti-effeminacy attitudes. Journal of Homosexuality, 42, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2013a). Accurate identification of a preference for insertive versus receptive intercourse from static facial cues of gay men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1217–1222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2013b). Accuracy in categorizing perceptually ambiguous groups: A review and meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 72–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, C. D., Fraccaro, P. J., Smith, F. G., Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., et al. (2010a). Taller men are less sensitive to cues of dominance in other men. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 943–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, C. D., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2010b). Individual differences in dominance perception: Dominant men are less sensitive to facial cues of male dominance. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 967–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegesin, D. J., & Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (2000). Top/bottom self-label, anal sex practices, HIV risk and gender role identity in gay men in New York City. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 12, 43–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinrich, J. D., Grant, I., Jacobson, D. L., Robinson, S. R., McCutchan, J. A., & the HNRC Group. (1992). Effect of recalled childhood gender non-conformity on adult genitoerotic role and AIDS exposure. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 559–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welling, L. L., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Smith, F. G., Feinberg, D. R., Little, A. C., et al. (2008). Men report stronger attraction to femininity in women’s faces when their testosterone levels are high. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 703–708.

  • Zebrowitz-McArthur, L., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward and ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zheng, L., Hart, T. A., & Zheng, Y. (2012). The relationship between intercourse preference positions and personality traits among gay men in China. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 683–689.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zheng, L., Hart, T. A., & Zheng, Y. (2013). Attraction to male facial masculinity in gay men in china: Relationship to intercourse preference positions and sociosexual behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1223–1232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, C., Raymond, H. F., Ding, X., Lu, R., Xu, J., Wu, G., … Shao, Y. (2013). Anal sex role, circumcision status, and HIV infection among men who have sex with men in Chongqing, China. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1275–1283.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Konstantin O. Tskhay.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tskhay, K.O., Re, D.E. & Rule, N.O. Individual Differences in Perceptions of Gay Men’s Sexual Role Preferences from Facial Cues. Arch Sex Behav 43, 1615–1620 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0319-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0319-x

Keywords

Navigation