Abstract
Research has demonstrated that the sexual role preferences of gay men can be perceived with accuracies that exceed chance guessing from viewing photos of their faces. This research was conducted with only heterosexual perceivers making the categorizations. We therefore examined whether men who have sex with men (N = 121) were able to perceive sexual role preferences from faces and, critically, whether perceivers’ masculinity, femininity, homonegativity, and own sexual role preference affected their categorizations of targets as “tops” or “bottoms.” We found that men who have sex with men, like heterosexual perceivers in prior work, perceived gay men’s sexual role preferences accurately. Furthermore, men who self-identified with a receptive (bottom) role were more accurate in their categorizations and male perceivers who self-reported higher levels of masculinity were more likely to categorize other men as bottoms. These findings suggest that men’s masculinity could serve as a lens through which people perceive others and interact with the world.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
Bisexual men did not differ from gay men on any measures of interest.
The hit and the false alarm rates were used in the calculations of accuracy and bias. Thus, we do not focus on the separate effects for these variables in our discussion. However, more masculine people had both greater hit and false alarm rates, suggesting the possibility of a response bias.
Because the sample of bisexual men was too small for any statistical inference, we examined gay men as a separate sample and found effects that were similar in magnitude and direction. All patterns of statistical significance were the same.
References
Andrzejewski, S. A., Hall, J. A., & Salib, E. R. (2009). Outgroup prejudice and accuracy of identifying outgroup membership: The case of anti-Semitism. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 47–58.
Archer, J. (1991). The influence of testosterone on human aggression. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 1–28.
Bailey, J. M., Kim, P. Y., Hills, A., & Linsenmeier, J. A. W. (1997). Butch, femme, or straight acting? Partner preferences of gay men and lesbians. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 960–973.
Bem, S. L. (1981). Bem sex-role inventory: Professional manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Buhrmester, M. D., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5.
Carballo-Diéguez, A., Dolezal, C., Nieves, L., Diaz, F., Decena, C., & Balan, I. (2004). Looking for a tall, dark, macho man … Sexual-role behavior variations in Latino gay and bisexual men. Culture, Health, & Sexuality, 6, 159–171.
Dabbs, J. M., & Mallinger, A. (1999). High testosterone levels predict low voice pitch among men. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 801–804.
Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Law Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., … Perrett, D. I. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 215–222.
Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., Karney, B. R., Reis, H. T., & Sprecher, S. (2012). Online dating: A critical analysis from the perspective of psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13, 3–66.
Gil, S. (2007). A narrative exploration of gay men’s sexual practices as a dialectical dialogue. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 22, 63–75.
Hall, J. A., Rosip, J. C., Smith LeBeau, L., Horgan, T. G., & Carter, J. D. (2006). Attributing the sources of accuracy in unequal-power dyadic communication: Who is better and why? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 18–27.
Han, C. S. (2008). A qualitative exploration of the relationship between racism and unsafe sex among Asian Pacific Islander gay men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 827–837.
Hart, T. A., Wolitski, R. J., Purcell, D. W., Gómez, C., & The Seropositive Urban Men’s Study Team. (2003). Sexual behavior among HIV-positive men who have sex with men: What’s in a label? Journal of Sex Research, 40, 179–188.
Kippax, S., & Smith, G. (2001). Anal intercourse and power in sex between men. Sexualities, 4, 413–434.
Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., & Keltner, D. (2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 1716–1723.
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353–363.
Mignault, A., & Chaudhuri, A. (2003). The many faces of a neutral face: Head tilt and perception of dominance and emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 111–132.
Morrison, M. A., & Morrison, T. G. (2003). Development and validation of a scale measuring modern prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women. Journal of Homosexuality, 43, 15–37.
Moskowitz, D. A., & Hart, T. A. (2011). The influence of physical body traits and masculinity on anal sex roles in gay and bisexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 835–841.
Moskowitz, D. A., Rieger, G., & Roloff, M. E. (2008). Tops, bottoms and versatiles. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 23, 191–202.
Moskowitz, D. A., Rieger, G., & Seal, D. W. (2009). Narcissism, self-evaluation, and partner preferences among men who have sex with men. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 725–728.
Pachankis, J. E., Buttenwieser, I. G., Bernstein, L. B., & Bayles, D. O. (2013). A longitudinal, mixed methods study of sexual position identity, behavior, and fantasies among young sexual minority men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1241–1253.
Penton-Voak, I. S., & Chen, J. Y. (2004). High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 229–241.
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–887.
Raymond, H. F., & McFarland, W. (2009). An examination of the rates of receptive anal intercourse among Asian/Pacific Islander men who have sex with men in San Francisco. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 168–169.
Rieger, G., Linsenmeier, J. A. W., Gygax, L., Garcia, S., & Bailey, J. M. (2010). Dissecting “gaydar”: Accuracy and the role of masculinity-femininity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 124–140.
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (2008). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Rule, N. O., Ishii, K., Ambady, N., Rosen, K. S., & Hallett, K. C. (2011). Found in translation: Cross-cultural consensus in the accurate categorization of male sexual orientation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1449–1507.
Tan, J. Y., Pratto, F., Operario, D., & Dworkin, S. L. (2013). Sexual positioning and race-based attraction by preferences for social dominance among gay Asian/Pacific Islander men in the United States. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1233–1239.
Taywaditep, K. J. (2002). Marginalization among the marginalized: Gay men’s anti-effeminacy attitudes. Journal of Homosexuality, 42, 1–28.
Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2013a). Accurate identification of a preference for insertive versus receptive intercourse from static facial cues of gay men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1217–1222.
Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2013b). Accuracy in categorizing perceptually ambiguous groups: A review and meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 72–86.
Watkins, C. D., Fraccaro, P. J., Smith, F. G., Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., et al. (2010a). Taller men are less sensitive to cues of dominance in other men. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 943–947.
Watkins, C. D., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2010b). Individual differences in dominance perception: Dominant men are less sensitive to facial cues of male dominance. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 967–971.
Wegesin, D. J., & Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (2000). Top/bottom self-label, anal sex practices, HIV risk and gender role identity in gay men in New York City. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 12, 43–62.
Weinrich, J. D., Grant, I., Jacobson, D. L., Robinson, S. R., McCutchan, J. A., & the HNRC Group. (1992). Effect of recalled childhood gender non-conformity on adult genitoerotic role and AIDS exposure. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 559–585.
Welling, L. L., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Smith, F. G., Feinberg, D. R., Little, A. C., et al. (2008). Men report stronger attraction to femininity in women’s faces when their testosterone levels are high. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 703–708.
Zebrowitz-McArthur, L., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward and ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215–238.
Zheng, L., Hart, T. A., & Zheng, Y. (2012). The relationship between intercourse preference positions and personality traits among gay men in China. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 683–689.
Zheng, L., Hart, T. A., & Zheng, Y. (2013). Attraction to male facial masculinity in gay men in china: Relationship to intercourse preference positions and sociosexual behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1223–1232.
Zhou, C., Raymond, H. F., Ding, X., Lu, R., Xu, J., Wu, G., … Shao, Y. (2013). Anal sex role, circumcision status, and HIV infection among men who have sex with men in Chongqing, China. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1275–1283.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Tskhay, K.O., Re, D.E. & Rule, N.O. Individual Differences in Perceptions of Gay Men’s Sexual Role Preferences from Facial Cues. Arch Sex Behav 43, 1615–1620 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0319-x
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0319-x