Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Evidence of Cognitive Biases for Maximizing Indirect Fitness in Samoan Fa’afafine

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The kin selection hypothesis posits that male androphilia (i.e., sexual attraction to males), although detrimental to reproduction (i.e., direct fitness), has persisted and evolved because androphilic males compensate by increasing their indirect fitness via increased investment in kin. In previous studies, Samoan androphilic males (known locally as fa’afafine) reported elevated avuncular (i.e., uncle-like) tendencies compared to Samoan gynephilic (i.e., sexually attracted to females) men. Their avuncular tendencies were also greater than the materteral (i.e., aunt-like) tendencies of androphilic women. The present study examined whether the avuncular cognition of fa’afafine was enhanced for maximizing indirect fitness. To do so, it examined whether fa’afafine had a stronger propensity than Samoan gynephilic men and androphilic women to invest in kin categories that would result in more reliable and substantive increases in indirect fitness (i.e., young, female kin) in hypothetical investment scenarios. In a forced-choice paradigm, although all individuals showed some degree of bias to invest in adaptive kin categories during non-frivolous investment contexts in which the consequences of investment were relatively non-trivial, fa’afafine showed greater adherence to the predicted pattern. In addition, shifting the context from frivolous investments, in which the consequences of investment were relatively trivial, to non-frivolous investments prompted fa’afafine to exhibit an enhanced preference, relative to Samoan gynephilic men and androphilic women, to invest in adaptive kin categories. These findings were consistent with the kin selection hypothesis and suggest that, although all individuals exhibit cognitive biases for increasing indirect fitness, the avuncular cognition of androphilic males has undergone selective enhancement to maximize the accrual of indirect fitness via kin-directed altruism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alanko, K., Santtila, P., Harlaar, N., Witting, K., Varjoen, M., Jern, P., … Sandnabba, N. K. (2010). Common genetic effects of gender atypical behavior in childhood and sexual orientation in adulthood: A study of Finnish twins. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 81–92.

  • Bailey, J. M. (2003). The man who would be queen: The science of gender-bending and transsexualism. Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, J. M., Dunne, M. P., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Genetics and environmental influences on sexual orientation and its correlates in an Australian twin sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 524–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belovsky, G. E. (1988). An optimal foraging-based model of hunter-gather population dynamics. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 7, 329–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betzig, L. (2012). Means, variances, and ranges in reproductive success: Comparative evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 309–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betzig, L. L., & Turke, P. W. (1986). Food sharing on Ifaluk. Current Anthropology, 27, 397–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard, R. (2012). Fertility in the mothers of firstborn homosexual and heterosexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 551–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard, R., & Lippa, R. A. (2007). Birth order, sibling sex ratio, handedness, and sexual orientation of male and female participants in a BBC Internet research project. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 163–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bobrow, D., & Bailey, J. M. (2001). Is male homosexuality maintained via kin selection? Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 361–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camperio Ciani, A., Corna, F., & Capiluppi, C. (2004). Evidence for maternally inherited factors favoring male homosexuality and promoting female fecundity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, 2217–2221.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daly, M., Salmon, C., & Wilson, M. (1997). Kinship: The conceptual hole in psychological studies of social cognition and close relationships. In J. A. Simpson & D. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 265–296). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.). Boston: PWS Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forrester, D. L., VanderLaan, D. P., Parker, J. L., & Vasey, P. L. (2011). Male sexual orientation and avuncularity in Canada: Implications for the kin selection hypothesis. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11, 339–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaulin, S. J. C., McBurney, D. H., & Brakeman-Warell, S. L. (1997). Matrilateral biases in the investment of aunts and uncles: A consequence and measure of paternity uncertainty. Human Nature, 8, 139–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1963). The evolution of altruistic behavior. American Naturalist, 97, 354–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, L. D. (1987). Quest for the real Samoa: The Mead/Freeman controversy and beyond. South Hadley, MA: Bergin and Garvey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, N. (1979). Demography of the Dobe !Kung. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iemmola, F., & Camperio Ciani, A. (2009). New evidence of genetic factors influencing sexual orientation in men: Female fecundity increase in the maternal line. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 393–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., Gilman, S. E., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Sexual orientation in a US national sample of twin and non-twin sibling pairs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1843–1846.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • King, M. D., Green, J., Osborn, D. P. J., Arkell, J., Hetherton, J., & Pereira, E. (2005). Family size in white gay and heterosexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 117–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkpatrick, R. C. (2000). The evolution of human homosexual behavior. Current Anthropology, 41, 385–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Långström, N., Rahman, Q., Carlström, E., & Lichtenstein, P. (2010). Genetic and environmental effects on same-sex sexual behavior: A population study of twins in Sweden. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 75–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeVay, S. (2011). Gay, straight, and the reason why: The science of sexual orientation. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manson, J., & Wrangham, R. W. (1991). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Current Anthropology, 32, 369–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nardi, B. A. (1984). Infant feeding and women’s work in Western Samoa: A hypothesis, some evidence and suggestions for future research. Ecology of Food and Nutrition, 13(14), 277–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rahman, Q., Collins, A., Morrison, M., Orrells, J. C., Cadinouche, K., Greenfield, S., & Begum, S. (2008). Maternal inheritance and familial fecundity factors in male homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 962–969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rahman, Q., & Hull, M. S. (2005). An empirical test of the kin selection hypothesis for male homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 962–969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saville, D. J. (1990). Multiple comparison procedures: The practical solution. American Statistician, 44, 174–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, G., Kim, R. M., Kolundziji, A. B., Rieger, G., & Sanders, A. R. (2010). Biodemographic and physical correlates of sexual orientation in men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 93–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sear, R., Steele, F., McGregor, I. A., & Mace, R. (2002). The effects of kin on child mortality in rural Gambia. Demography, 39, 43–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shore, B. (1978). Ghosts and government: A structural analysis of alternative institutions for conflict management in Samoa. Man, 13, 175–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. S., Kish, B. J., & Crawford, C. B. (1987). Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethology and Sociobiolgy, 8, 171–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of Man 1871-1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L., & Willard, D. E. (1973). Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science, 179, 90–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VanderLaan, D. P., Forrester, D. L., Petterson, L. J., & Vasey, P. L. (2012). Offspring production among the extended relatives of Samoan men and fa’afafine. PLoS ONE. e36088.

  • VanderLaan, D. P., Forrester, D. L., Petterson, L. J., & Vasey, P. L. (2013a). The prevalence of fa’afafine relatives among Samoan gynephilic men and fa’afafine. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 353–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VanderLaan, D. P., Gothreau, L. M., Bartlett, N. H., & Vasey, P. L. (2011). Separation anxiety in feminine boys: pathological or prosocial? Journal of Gay and Lesbian Mental Health, 15, 30–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanderLaan, D. P., Ren, Z., & Vasey, P. L. (2013b). Male androphilia in the ancestral past: An ethnological analysis. Human Nature, 24, 375–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VanderLaan, D. P., & Vasey, P. L. (2011). Male sexual orientation in Independent Samoa: Evidence for fraternal birth order and maternal fecundity effects. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 495–503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VanderLaan, D. P., & Vasey, P. L. (2012). Relationship status and elevated avuncularity in Samoan fa’afafine. Personal Relationships, 19, 326–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanderLaan, D. P., & Vasey, P. L. (2013). Birth order and avuncular tendencies in Samoan men and fa’afafine. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 371–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VanderLaan, D. P., Vokey, J. R., & Vasey, P. L. (2013c). Is transgendered male androphilia familial in non-Western populations? The case of a Samoan village. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 361–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., Parker, J. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2014). Comparative reproductive output of androphilic and gynephilic males in Samoa. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 363–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., Pocock, D. S., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2007). Kin selection and male androphilia in Samoan fa’afafine. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 159–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2007). Birth order and male androphilia in Samoan fa’afafine. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 274, 1437–1442.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2009). Materteral and avuncular tendencies in Samoa: A comparative study of women, men, and fa’afafine. Human Nature, 20, 269–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010a). Avuncular tendencies in Samoan fa’afafine and the evolution of male androphilia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 821–830.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010b). An adaptive cognitive dissociation between willingness to help kin and non-kin in Samoan fa’afafine. Psychological Science, 21, 292–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010c). Monetary exchanges with nieces and nephews: A comparison of Samoan men, women, and fa’afafine. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 371–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2012). Sexual orientation in men and avuncularity in Japan: Implications for the kin selection hypothesis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 209–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. P. (1998). Coalitionary mate guarding by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 44, 43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, W. L. (1992). The spirit and the flesh: Sexual diversity in American Indian culture. Boston: Beacon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, G., & Rahman, Q. (2005). Born gay: The psychobiology of sex orientation. London: Peter Owen Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Resitara Apa, Nancy Bartlett, Surindar Cheema, Gardenia Elisala, Vaosa Epa, Fitu Fuimaono, Valella Iosua, Anita Latai, Tyrone Laurenson, Jeannette Mageo, Gaualofa Matalavea, Avau Junior Memea, Sam Pania, Nella Tavita-Levy, Palanitina Toelupe, Trisha Tuiloma, Avalogo Togi A. Tunupopo, Erin Zelinski, the Kuka family of Savai’i, the National University of Samoa, the Samoan AIDS Foundation, the National University of Samoa, the Samoan Fa’afafine Association, the Government of Samoa, and all individuals who participated in our study. We also thank the Editor and three anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments and suggestions on previous versions of this paper. We extend special thanks to Alatina Ioelu without whose help this study would not have been possible. This research was supported by the University of Lethbridge, by a NSERC of Canada Graduate Scholarship-D3, a Henry David Travel Grant, and a Ralph Steinhauer Award of Distinction to DPV as well as by a NSERC of Canada Discovery Grant to PLV.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Doug P. VanderLaan.

Appendix

Appendix

Within-Group Frivolous Investment Context Choice Patterns

The following analyses pertain to within-group preferences for sisters’ sons, sisters’ daughters, brothers’ sons, and brothers’ daughters (Fig. 1). For frivolous time investments, men preferred all kin categories to brothers’ daughters (ps < .001). For frivolous monetary investments, men preferred sisters’ sons to all other kin categories (sisters’ sons > brothers’ sons, p = .002; sisters’ sons > sisters’ daughters and brothers’ daughters, p < .001). Additionally, for frivolous monetary investments, men preferred brothers’ daughters less than brothers’ sons (p < .001) and sisters’ daughters (p = .001). For frivolous time investments, women preferred sisters’ daughters to brothers’ daughters (p = .03) and brothers’ sons (p = .002). For frivolous monetary investments, women preferred brothers’ daughters less than brothers’ sons (p = .008) and sisters’ daughters (p < .001). For frivolous time investments, fa’afafine preferred sisters’ to brothers’ children (sisters’ daughters and sisters’ sons > brothers’ sons, p = .01; sisters’ sons > brothers’ daughters, p = .01; sisters’ daughters > brothers’ daughters, p < .001). For frivolous monetary investments, fa’afafine preferred brothers’ daughters less than brothers’ sons, sisters’ daughters (ps = .001), and sisters’ sons (p < .001).

The following analyses pertain to within-group preferences for older siblings’ sons, older siblings’ daughters, younger siblings’ sons, and younger siblings’ daughters (Fig. 2). For frivolous time investments, men preferred older siblings’ sons to all other groups (ps < .001). For frivolous monetary investments, men preferred older siblings’ sons over all other categories (ps < .001) and older siblings’ daughters over younger siblings’ daughters (p = .02). For frivolous time investments, women preferred younger siblings’ sons less than younger siblings’ daughters (p = .04) and older siblings’ daughters (p = .03). For frivolous monetary investments, women showed no significant preference. For frivolous monetary investments, fa’afafine preferred older siblings’ sons to younger siblings’ sons (p = .01) and older siblings’ daughters (p < .001) as well as younger siblings’ daughters to older siblings’ daughters (p = .02).

Between-Group Comparisons of Frivolous Investment Context Choice Patterns

The following analyses pertain to between-group differences in preferences for sisters’ sons, sisters’ daughters, brothers’ sons, and brothers’ daughters (Fig. 4). For frivolous time investments, men showed a greater preference than women for sisters’ sons and brothers’ sons (ps = .02) and a lesser preference for sisters’ daughters than women (p = .05) and fa’afafine (p = .04). In contrast, women showed a greater preference for brothers’ daughters than men (p = .001) and fa’afafine (p = .05). For frivolous monetary investments, men preferred sisters’ sons relative to women (p < .001) and fa’afafine (p = .003). Also, women preferred sisters’ daughters relative to men (p = .006).

The following analyses pertain to between-group differences in preferences for older siblings’ sons, older siblings’ daughters, younger siblings’ sons, and younger siblings’ daughters (Fig. 5). For frivolous time investments, men chose older siblings’ sons more than women (p < .001) and fa’afafine (p = .001), and, compared to men, women preferred older siblings’ daughters (p = .02) and younger siblings’ daughters (p = .02). For frivolous monetary investments, men chose older siblings’ sons more than women (p = .002) and younger siblings’ daughters less than women (p = .005) and fa’afafine (p = .01).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

VanderLaan, D.P., Vasey, P.L. Evidence of Cognitive Biases for Maximizing Indirect Fitness in Samoan Fa’afafine . Arch Sex Behav 43, 1009–1022 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0288-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0288-0

Keywords

Navigation