Abstract
The kin selection hypothesis posits that male androphilia (i.e., sexual attraction to males), although detrimental to reproduction (i.e., direct fitness), has persisted and evolved because androphilic males compensate by increasing their indirect fitness via increased investment in kin. In previous studies, Samoan androphilic males (known locally as fa’afafine) reported elevated avuncular (i.e., uncle-like) tendencies compared to Samoan gynephilic (i.e., sexually attracted to females) men. Their avuncular tendencies were also greater than the materteral (i.e., aunt-like) tendencies of androphilic women. The present study examined whether the avuncular cognition of fa’afafine was enhanced for maximizing indirect fitness. To do so, it examined whether fa’afafine had a stronger propensity than Samoan gynephilic men and androphilic women to invest in kin categories that would result in more reliable and substantive increases in indirect fitness (i.e., young, female kin) in hypothetical investment scenarios. In a forced-choice paradigm, although all individuals showed some degree of bias to invest in adaptive kin categories during non-frivolous investment contexts in which the consequences of investment were relatively non-trivial, fa’afafine showed greater adherence to the predicted pattern. In addition, shifting the context from frivolous investments, in which the consequences of investment were relatively trivial, to non-frivolous investments prompted fa’afafine to exhibit an enhanced preference, relative to Samoan gynephilic men and androphilic women, to invest in adaptive kin categories. These findings were consistent with the kin selection hypothesis and suggest that, although all individuals exhibit cognitive biases for increasing indirect fitness, the avuncular cognition of androphilic males has undergone selective enhancement to maximize the accrual of indirect fitness via kin-directed altruism.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Alanko, K., Santtila, P., Harlaar, N., Witting, K., Varjoen, M., Jern, P., … Sandnabba, N. K. (2010). Common genetic effects of gender atypical behavior in childhood and sexual orientation in adulthood: A study of Finnish twins. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 81–92.
Bailey, J. M. (2003). The man who would be queen: The science of gender-bending and transsexualism. Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press.
Bailey, J. M., Dunne, M. P., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Genetics and environmental influences on sexual orientation and its correlates in an Australian twin sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 524–536.
Belovsky, G. E. (1988). An optimal foraging-based model of hunter-gather population dynamics. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 7, 329–372.
Betzig, L. (2012). Means, variances, and ranges in reproductive success: Comparative evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 309–317.
Betzig, L. L., & Turke, P. W. (1986). Food sharing on Ifaluk. Current Anthropology, 27, 397–400.
Blanchard, R. (2012). Fertility in the mothers of firstborn homosexual and heterosexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 551–556.
Blanchard, R., & Lippa, R. A. (2007). Birth order, sibling sex ratio, handedness, and sexual orientation of male and female participants in a BBC Internet research project. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 163–176.
Bobrow, D., & Bailey, J. M. (2001). Is male homosexuality maintained via kin selection? Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 361–368.
Camperio Ciani, A., Corna, F., & Capiluppi, C. (2004). Evidence for maternally inherited factors favoring male homosexuality and promoting female fecundity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, 2217–2221.
Daly, M., Salmon, C., & Wilson, M. (1997). Kinship: The conceptual hole in psychological studies of social cognition and close relationships. In J. A. Simpson & D. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 265–296). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.). Boston: PWS Publishers.
Forrester, D. L., VanderLaan, D. P., Parker, J. L., & Vasey, P. L. (2011). Male sexual orientation and avuncularity in Canada: Implications for the kin selection hypothesis. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11, 339–352.
Gaulin, S. J. C., McBurney, D. H., & Brakeman-Warell, S. L. (1997). Matrilateral biases in the investment of aunts and uncles: A consequence and measure of paternity uncertainty. Human Nature, 8, 139–151.
Hamilton, W. D. (1963). The evolution of altruistic behavior. American Naturalist, 97, 354–356.
Holmes, L. D. (1987). Quest for the real Samoa: The Mead/Freeman controversy and beyond. South Hadley, MA: Bergin and Garvey.
Howell, N. (1979). Demography of the Dobe !Kung. New York: Academic Press.
Iemmola, F., & Camperio Ciani, A. (2009). New evidence of genetic factors influencing sexual orientation in men: Female fecundity increase in the maternal line. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 393–399.
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., Gilman, S. E., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Sexual orientation in a US national sample of twin and non-twin sibling pairs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1843–1846.
King, M. D., Green, J., Osborn, D. P. J., Arkell, J., Hetherton, J., & Pereira, E. (2005). Family size in white gay and heterosexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 117–122.
Kirkpatrick, R. C. (2000). The evolution of human homosexual behavior. Current Anthropology, 41, 385–413.
Långström, N., Rahman, Q., Carlström, E., & Lichtenstein, P. (2010). Genetic and environmental effects on same-sex sexual behavior: A population study of twins in Sweden. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 75–80.
LeVay, S. (2011). Gay, straight, and the reason why: The science of sexual orientation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Manson, J., & Wrangham, R. W. (1991). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Current Anthropology, 32, 369–390.
Nardi, B. A. (1984). Infant feeding and women’s work in Western Samoa: A hypothesis, some evidence and suggestions for future research. Ecology of Food and Nutrition, 13(14), 277–286.
Rahman, Q., Collins, A., Morrison, M., Orrells, J. C., Cadinouche, K., Greenfield, S., & Begum, S. (2008). Maternal inheritance and familial fecundity factors in male homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 962–969.
Rahman, Q., & Hull, M. S. (2005). An empirical test of the kin selection hypothesis for male homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 962–969.
Saville, D. J. (1990). Multiple comparison procedures: The practical solution. American Statistician, 44, 174–180.
Schwartz, G., Kim, R. M., Kolundziji, A. B., Rieger, G., & Sanders, A. R. (2010). Biodemographic and physical correlates of sexual orientation in men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 93–109.
Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 1–18.
Sear, R., Steele, F., McGregor, I. A., & Mace, R. (2002). The effects of kin on child mortality in rural Gambia. Demography, 39, 43–63.
Shore, B. (1978). Ghosts and government: A structural analysis of alternative institutions for conflict management in Samoa. Man, 13, 175–199.
Smith, M. S., Kish, B. J., & Crawford, C. B. (1987). Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethology and Sociobiolgy, 8, 171–182.
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of Man 1871-1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Trivers, R. L., & Willard, D. E. (1973). Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science, 179, 90–92.
VanderLaan, D. P., Forrester, D. L., Petterson, L. J., & Vasey, P. L. (2012). Offspring production among the extended relatives of Samoan men and fa’afafine. PLoS ONE. e36088.
VanderLaan, D. P., Forrester, D. L., Petterson, L. J., & Vasey, P. L. (2013a). The prevalence of fa’afafine relatives among Samoan gynephilic men and fa’afafine. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 353–359.
VanderLaan, D. P., Gothreau, L. M., Bartlett, N. H., & Vasey, P. L. (2011). Separation anxiety in feminine boys: pathological or prosocial? Journal of Gay and Lesbian Mental Health, 15, 30–45.
VanderLaan, D. P., Ren, Z., & Vasey, P. L. (2013b). Male androphilia in the ancestral past: An ethnological analysis. Human Nature, 24, 375–401.
VanderLaan, D. P., & Vasey, P. L. (2011). Male sexual orientation in Independent Samoa: Evidence for fraternal birth order and maternal fecundity effects. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 495–503.
VanderLaan, D. P., & Vasey, P. L. (2012). Relationship status and elevated avuncularity in Samoan fa’afafine. Personal Relationships, 19, 326–339.
VanderLaan, D. P., & Vasey, P. L. (2013). Birth order and avuncular tendencies in Samoan men and fa’afafine. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 371–379.
VanderLaan, D. P., Vokey, J. R., & Vasey, P. L. (2013c). Is transgendered male androphilia familial in non-Western populations? The case of a Samoan village. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 361–370.
Vasey, P. L., Parker, J. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2014). Comparative reproductive output of androphilic and gynephilic males in Samoa. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 363–367.
Vasey, P. L., Pocock, D. S., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2007). Kin selection and male androphilia in Samoan fa’afafine. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 159–167.
Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2007). Birth order and male androphilia in Samoan fa’afafine. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 274, 1437–1442.
Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2009). Materteral and avuncular tendencies in Samoa: A comparative study of women, men, and fa’afafine. Human Nature, 20, 269–281.
Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010a). Avuncular tendencies in Samoan fa’afafine and the evolution of male androphilia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 821–830.
Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010b). An adaptive cognitive dissociation between willingness to help kin and non-kin in Samoan fa’afafine. Psychological Science, 21, 292–297.
Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010c). Monetary exchanges with nieces and nephews: A comparison of Samoan men, women, and fa’afafine. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 371–380.
Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2012). Sexual orientation in men and avuncularity in Japan: Implications for the kin selection hypothesis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 209–215.
Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182–192.
Watts, D. P. (1998). Coalitionary mate guarding by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 44, 43–55.
Williams, W. L. (1992). The spirit and the flesh: Sexual diversity in American Indian culture. Boston: Beacon Press.
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
Wilson, G., & Rahman, Q. (2005). Born gay: The psychobiology of sex orientation. London: Peter Owen Publishers.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Resitara Apa, Nancy Bartlett, Surindar Cheema, Gardenia Elisala, Vaosa Epa, Fitu Fuimaono, Valella Iosua, Anita Latai, Tyrone Laurenson, Jeannette Mageo, Gaualofa Matalavea, Avau Junior Memea, Sam Pania, Nella Tavita-Levy, Palanitina Toelupe, Trisha Tuiloma, Avalogo Togi A. Tunupopo, Erin Zelinski, the Kuka family of Savai’i, the National University of Samoa, the Samoan AIDS Foundation, the National University of Samoa, the Samoan Fa’afafine Association, the Government of Samoa, and all individuals who participated in our study. We also thank the Editor and three anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments and suggestions on previous versions of this paper. We extend special thanks to Alatina Ioelu without whose help this study would not have been possible. This research was supported by the University of Lethbridge, by a NSERC of Canada Graduate Scholarship-D3, a Henry David Travel Grant, and a Ralph Steinhauer Award of Distinction to DPV as well as by a NSERC of Canada Discovery Grant to PLV.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendix
Appendix
Within-Group Frivolous Investment Context Choice Patterns
The following analyses pertain to within-group preferences for sisters’ sons, sisters’ daughters, brothers’ sons, and brothers’ daughters (Fig. 1). For frivolous time investments, men preferred all kin categories to brothers’ daughters (ps < .001). For frivolous monetary investments, men preferred sisters’ sons to all other kin categories (sisters’ sons > brothers’ sons, p = .002; sisters’ sons > sisters’ daughters and brothers’ daughters, p < .001). Additionally, for frivolous monetary investments, men preferred brothers’ daughters less than brothers’ sons (p < .001) and sisters’ daughters (p = .001). For frivolous time investments, women preferred sisters’ daughters to brothers’ daughters (p = .03) and brothers’ sons (p = .002). For frivolous monetary investments, women preferred brothers’ daughters less than brothers’ sons (p = .008) and sisters’ daughters (p < .001). For frivolous time investments, fa’afafine preferred sisters’ to brothers’ children (sisters’ daughters and sisters’ sons > brothers’ sons, p = .01; sisters’ sons > brothers’ daughters, p = .01; sisters’ daughters > brothers’ daughters, p < .001). For frivolous monetary investments, fa’afafine preferred brothers’ daughters less than brothers’ sons, sisters’ daughters (ps = .001), and sisters’ sons (p < .001).
The following analyses pertain to within-group preferences for older siblings’ sons, older siblings’ daughters, younger siblings’ sons, and younger siblings’ daughters (Fig. 2). For frivolous time investments, men preferred older siblings’ sons to all other groups (ps < .001). For frivolous monetary investments, men preferred older siblings’ sons over all other categories (ps < .001) and older siblings’ daughters over younger siblings’ daughters (p = .02). For frivolous time investments, women preferred younger siblings’ sons less than younger siblings’ daughters (p = .04) and older siblings’ daughters (p = .03). For frivolous monetary investments, women showed no significant preference. For frivolous monetary investments, fa’afafine preferred older siblings’ sons to younger siblings’ sons (p = .01) and older siblings’ daughters (p < .001) as well as younger siblings’ daughters to older siblings’ daughters (p = .02).
Between-Group Comparisons of Frivolous Investment Context Choice Patterns
The following analyses pertain to between-group differences in preferences for sisters’ sons, sisters’ daughters, brothers’ sons, and brothers’ daughters (Fig. 4). For frivolous time investments, men showed a greater preference than women for sisters’ sons and brothers’ sons (ps = .02) and a lesser preference for sisters’ daughters than women (p = .05) and fa’afafine (p = .04). In contrast, women showed a greater preference for brothers’ daughters than men (p = .001) and fa’afafine (p = .05). For frivolous monetary investments, men preferred sisters’ sons relative to women (p < .001) and fa’afafine (p = .003). Also, women preferred sisters’ daughters relative to men (p = .006).
The following analyses pertain to between-group differences in preferences for older siblings’ sons, older siblings’ daughters, younger siblings’ sons, and younger siblings’ daughters (Fig. 5). For frivolous time investments, men chose older siblings’ sons more than women (p < .001) and fa’afafine (p = .001), and, compared to men, women preferred older siblings’ daughters (p = .02) and younger siblings’ daughters (p = .02). For frivolous monetary investments, men chose older siblings’ sons more than women (p = .002) and younger siblings’ daughters less than women (p = .005) and fa’afafine (p = .01).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
VanderLaan, D.P., Vasey, P.L. Evidence of Cognitive Biases for Maximizing Indirect Fitness in Samoan Fa’afafine . Arch Sex Behav 43, 1009–1022 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0288-0
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0288-0