Skip to main content
Log in

On the Relationship Between Automatic Attitudes and Self-Reported Sexual Assault in Men

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research and theory suggest rape supportive attitudes are important predictors of sexual assault; yet, to date, rape supportive attitudes have been assessed through self-report measures that are methodologically and theoretically limited. To address these limitations, the objectives of the current project were to: (1) develop a novel implicit rape attitude assessment that captures automatic attitudes about rape and does not rely on self-reports, and (2) examine the association between automatic rape attitudes and sexual assault perpetration. We predicted that automatic rape attitudes would be a significant unique predictor of sexual assault even when self-reported rape attitudes (i.e., rape myth acceptance and hostility toward women) were controlled. We tested the generalizability of this prediction in two independent samples: a sample of undergraduate college men (n = 75, M age = 19.3 years) and a sample of men from the community (n = 50, M age = 35.9 years). We found the novel implicit rape attitude assessment was significantly associated with the frequency of sexual assault perpetration in both samples and contributed unique variance in explaining sexual assault beyond rape myth acceptance and hostility toward women. We discuss the ways in which future research on automatic rape attitudes may significantly advance measurement and theory aimed at understanding and preventing sexual assault.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We applied a more stringent inclusion criteria by excluding participants with error rates over 25% on the priming procedure (n = 20) and reran all analyses. The results were unchanged when the more stringent criteria was used; thus, we chose to retain the larger sample.

  2. The most common forms of sexual assault occur between acquaintances and involve alcohol and/or verbal coercion (Koss et al., 1987); however, it was not possible to identify still-frame images that clearly depicted these forms of sexual assault. Instead, the images we located involved more overt forms of physical force. For the sake of simplicity, we will refer to these images as “rape images” for the remainder of the article.

  3. Although many attitude objects have a naturally contrasting comparison group where positivity toward one category implies negativity toward the other, rape has no obvious contrasting category (for a similar point, see Karpinski & Steinman, 2006). Thus, while we included filler images of related but distinct categories (i.e., sex and violence), we were primarily interested in responses latencies to the positive and negative words preceded by rape images.

  4. Research by Geer and Bellard (1996) and Geer and Melton (1997) indicates that respondents are generally slower to respond to erotic words in lexical decision tasks, a phenomenon which may be due to the taboo nature of sexual content. This “sexual content-induced delay” may have occurred in the present research, but would have had minimal impact on the attitude estimates we derive and relate to participant behavior. This is because differences between participants’ response latencies to positive versus negative targets (which had no sexual content themselves) preceded by the same prime are used in computing the attitude estimates. Thus, any slowing effects in response to sexual content would be canceled out in these comparative analyses.

References

  • Abbey, A., & Jacques-Tiura, A. J. (2011). Sexual assault perpetrators’ tactics: Associations with their personal characteristics and aspects of the incident. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 2866–2889.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abbey, A., McAuslan, P., & Ross, L. T. (1998). Sexual assault perpetration by college men: The role of alcohol, misperception of sexual intent, and sexual beliefs and experiences. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17, 167–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abbey, A., Parkhill, M. R., BeShears, R., Clinton-Sherrod, A. M., & Zawacki, T. (2006). Cross-sectional predictors of sexual assault perpetration in a community sample of single African American and Caucasian men. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 54–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abbey, A., Parkhill, M. R., & Koss, M. P. (2005). The effects of frame of reference on responses to questions about sexual assault victimization and perpetration. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 364–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abbey, A., Zawacki, T., Buck, P. O., & McAuslan, P. (2004). Sexual assault and alcohol consumption: What do we know about their relationship and what types of research are still needed? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 271–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, L. A., & Whiston, S. C. (2005). Sexual assault education programs: A meta-analytic examination of their effectiveness. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 374–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkins, D. C., & Gallop, R. J. (2007). Rethinking how family researchers model infrequent outcomes: A tutorial on count regression and zero-inflated models. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 726–735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J. B., & Strack, F. (1995). Attractiveness of the underling: An automatic power-sex association and its consequences for sexual harassment and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 181–768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brecklin, L. R., & Forde, D. R. (2001). A meta-analysis of rape education programs. Violence and Victims, 16, 303–321.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brener, N. D., McMahon, P. W., Warren, C. W., & Douglas, K. A. (1999). Forced sexual intercourse and associated health-risk behaviors among female college students in the United States. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 252–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. S., Gray, N. S., & Snowden, R. J. (2009). Implicit measurement of sexual associations in child sex abusers. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21, 166–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, M. R. (1980). Cultural myths and supports for rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 217–230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., & Malamuth, N. (1996). Sex, power, conflict: Evolutionary and feminist perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calhoun, K. S., Bernat, J. A., Clum, G. A., & Fame, C. L. (1997). Sexual coercion and attraction to sexual aggression in a community sample of young men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12, 392–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. C. (2002). Health consequences of intimate partner violence. Lancet, 359, 1331–1336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Check, J. V. P., Malamuth, N. M., Elias, B., & Barton, S. (1985, April). On hostile ground. Psychology Today, pp. 56–61.

  • DeGue, S., & DiLillo, D. (2004). Understanding perpetrators of nonphysical sexual coercion: Characteristics of those who cross the line. Violence and Victims, 19, 673–688.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeGue, S., DiLillo, D., & Scalora, M. (2010). Are all perpetrators alike? Comparing risk factors for sexual coercion and aggression. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 22, 402–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demare, D., Briere, J., & Lips, H. M. (1988). Violent pornography and self-reported likelihood of sexual aggression. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 140–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice in interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 62–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunst, K. J., Hamby, D. W., & Trivette, C. M. (2004). Guidelines for calculating effect sizes for practice-based research synthesis. Centerscope, 3, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In C. Hendrick & M. S. Clack (Eds.), Research methods in personality and social psychology (pp. 74–97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 247–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (2000). Accessible attitudes as tools for object appraisal: Their costs and benefits. In G. R. Maio & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Functions of attitudes (pp. 1–36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flores, S. A., & Hartlaub, M. G. (1998). Reducing rape-myth acceptance in male college students: A meta analysis of intervention studies. Journal of College Student Development, 39, 438–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forbes, G. B., & Adams-Curtis, L. E. (2001). Experiences with sexual coercion in college males and females: Role of family conflict, sexist attitudes, acceptance of rape myths, self-esteem, and the big-five personality factors. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 865–889.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forbes, G. B., Adams-Curtis, L. E., & White, K. B. (2004). First- and second-generation measures of sexism, rape myths and related beliefs, and hostility toward women: Their interrelationships and association with college students’ experiences with dating aggression and sexual coercion. Violence Against Women, 10, 236–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geer, J. H., & Bellard, H. S. (1996). Sexual content induced delays in lexical decisions: Gender and context effects. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 379–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geer, J. H., & Melton, J. S. (1997). Sexual content induced delays with double-entendre words. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26, 295–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G. C. N., & Hirschman, R. (1994). The relationship between men’s sexual aggression inside and outside the laboratory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 375–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, J. A., & White, J. W. (2000). Women’s vulnerability to sexual assault from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27, 419–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, B. G. (2006). MediaLab Research Software, Version 2006 [computer program]. New York: Empirisoft.

  • Kamphuis, J. H., de Ruiter, C., Janssen, B., & Spiering, M. (2005). Preliminary evidence for an automatic link between sex and power among men who molest children. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 1351–1365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category Implicit Association Test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knight, R. A., & Sims-Knight, J. E. (2003). The developmental antecedents of sexual coercion against women in adolescents: Testing alternative hypotheses with structural equation modeling. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 989, 72–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koss, M. P., Abbey, A., Campbell, R., Cook, S., Norris, J., Testa, M., et al. (2007). Revising the SES: A collaborative process to improve assessment of sexual aggression and victimization. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 357–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koss, M. P., & Gidycz, C. A. (1985). Sexual experiences survey: Reliability and validity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 422–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koss, M. P., Gidycz, C. A., & Wisniewski, N. (1987). The scope of rape: Incidence and prevalence of sexual aggression and victimization in a national sample of higher education students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 162–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lanier, C. A. (2001). Rape-accepting attitudes: Precursors to or consequences of forced sex. Violence Against Women, 7, 876–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leibold, J. M., & McConnell, A. R. (2004). Women, sex, hostility, power, and suspicion: Sexually aggressive men’s cognitive associations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 256–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loh, C., Gidycz, C. A., Lobo, T. R., & Luthra, R. (2005). A prospective analysis of sexual assault perpetration: Risk factors related to perpetrator characteristics. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 1325–1348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1994). Rape myths: In review. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 133–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1995). Attitudinal antecedents of rape myth acceptance: A theoretical and empirical reexamination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 704–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malamuth, N. M. (2003). Criminal and noncriminal sexual aggressors: Integrating psychopathy in a hierarchical-mediational confluence model. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 989, 33–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mihailides, S., Devilly, G. J., & Ward, T. (2004). Implicit cognitive distortions and sexual offending. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16, 333–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D., Angelone, D. J., Hirschman, R., Lilly, R. S., & Hall, G. C. N. (2004). Peer modeling and college men’s sexually impositional behavior in the laboratory. Journal of Sex Research, 39, 222–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murnen, S. K., & Kohlman, M. H. (2007). Athletic participation, fraternity membership, and sexual aggression among college men: A meta-analytic review. Sex Roles, 57, 145–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murnen, S. K., Wright, C., & Kaluzny, G. (2002). If “boys will be boys,” then girls will be victims? A meta-analytic review of the research that relates masculine ideology to sexual aggression. Sex Roles, 46, 359–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 225–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, K. L., Firestones, P., & Baldwin, M. W. (2007). Indirect assessment of cognitions of child sexual abusers with the Implicit Association Test. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34, 454–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A. (2009). Measures of prejudice. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice (pp. 367–386). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 19–64). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overholser, J. C., & Beck, S. J. (1986). The classification of rapists and child molesters. Journal of Offender Counseling, Services, and Rehabilitation, 14, 169–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, D. L., Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1999). Rape myth acceptance: Exploration of its structure and its measurement using the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 27–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spitzberg, B. H. (1999). An analysis of empirical estimates of sexual aggression victimization and perpetration. Violence and Victims, 14, 241–260.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suarez, E., & Gadalla, T. M. (2010). Stop blaming the victim: A meta-analysis on rape myths. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25, 2010–2035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2000). Extent, nature, and consequences of intimate partner violence. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, NCJ 181867.

  • Travis, C. B. (2003). Evolution, gender, and rape. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Unkelbach, C., Fiedler, K., Bayer, M., Stegmüller, M., & Danner, D. (2008). Why positive information is processed faster: The density hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 36–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, J. W., & Frabutt, J. M. (2006). Violence against girls and women: An integrative developmental perspective. In J. Worell & C. D. Goodheart (Eds.), Handbook of girls’ and women’s psychological health (pp. 85–93). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widman, L. (2010). Inside the minds of sex offenders: Illuminating the role of implicit rape attitudes in sexual offending. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN.

  • Widman, L., Olson, M. A., & Bolen, R. (in press). Self-reported sexual assault in convicted sex offenders and community men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Cal Yeaman and Tiffany Smith for their assistance collecting data for this project. This research was supported in part by funding for Laura Widman from the American Psychological Association, the American Psychological Foundation, the Society the Psychological Study of Social Issues, and NIH/NIAID 5 T32 AI 07001-34: Training in Sexually Transmitted Diseases and HIV.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura Widman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Widman, L., Olson, M. On the Relationship Between Automatic Attitudes and Self-Reported Sexual Assault in Men. Arch Sex Behav 42, 813–823 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9970-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9970-2

Keywords

Navigation