Advertisement

Archives of Sexual Behavior

, Volume 41, Issue 1, pp 173–183 | Cite as

Sexual Fantasies and Viewing Times Across the Menstrual Cycle: A Diary Study

  • Samantha J. Dawson
  • Kelly D. Suschinsky
  • Martin L. LalumièreEmail author
Original Paper

Abstract

Recent research has revealed that many aspects of female sexuality change across the menstrual cycle. In this study, we examined changes in sexual fantasies and visual sexual interests across the menstrual cycle. A total of 27 single, heterosexual women (M age = 21.5 years) not using hormonal contraceptives answered questions on a web-based diary every day for 30 days about their sexual fantasies and behaviors. Twenty-two of them also completed a viewing time task during three different menstrual cycle phases (follicular, ovulation, and luteal) to assess changes in visual sexual interest. Ovulation status was determined by a self-administered urine test. Results showed that the frequency and arousability of sexual fantasies increased significantly at ovulation. The number of males in the fantasies increased during the most fertile period, with no such change for the number of females. Fantasy content became more female-like during ovulation, focusing more on emotions rather than explicit sexual content. Women displayed a category non-specific pattern of viewing time with regard to target age and gender, regardless of fertility status. Results were discussed in the context of the ovulatory shift hypothesis of female sexuality.

Keywords

Daily diary Sexuality Ovulation Menstrual cycle Sexual fantasies Viewing time 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Dean of Arts and Science, University of Lethbridge, for financial support, and Megan Ebsworth, Grant Harris, and Michael Seto for their useful comments. A version of this article was presented at the University of Lethbridge Workshop, The Puzzle of Sexual Orientation: What Is It and How Does It Work? Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada, June 2010.

References

  1. Barclay, A. M. (1973). Sexual fantasies in men and women. Medical Aspects of Sexuality, 7, 205–216.Google Scholar
  2. Bivona, J., & Critelli, J. (2009). The nature of women’s rape fantasies: An analysis of prevalence, frequency, and contents. Journal of Sex Research, 46, 33–45. doi: 10.1080/00224490802624406.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bond, S. B., & Mosher, D. L. (1986). Guided imagery of rape: Fantasy, reality, and the willing victim myth. Journal of Sex Research, 22, 162–183. doi: 10.1080/00224498609551298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bullivant, S. B., Sellergren, S. A., Stern, K., Spencer, N. A., Jacob, S., Mennella, J. A., & McClintock, M. K. (2004). Women’s sexual experience during the menstrual cycle: Identification of the sexual phase by noninvasive measurement of luteinizing hormone. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 82–93. doi: 10.1080/00224490409552216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cameron, P., & Biber, H. (1973). Sexual thought throughout the life-span. The Gerontologist, 13, 144–147. doi: 10.1093/geront/13.2.144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chen, J. Y. C. (2005). Changes in relationship quality across the female menstrual cycle: A diary study of dating couples. Unpublished master’s thesis, Texas A & M University, College Station, TX.Google Scholar
  7. Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, J. M. (2004). A sex difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological Science, 15, 736–744. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00750.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C., & Blanchard, R. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response to the sexual activities versus the gender of actors in sexual films. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1108–1121. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1108.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Diamond, L. M., & Wallen, K. (2011). Sexual minority women’s sexual motivation around the time of ovulation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 236–247. doi: 10.1007/s10508-010-9631-2.Google Scholar
  10. Ebsworth, M., & Lalumière, M. L. (2012). Viewing time as a measure of bisexual sexual interest. Archives of Sexual Behavior. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9923-9.
  11. Eisenman, R. (1982). Sexual behavior as related to sex fantasies and experimental manipulation of authoritarianism and creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 853–860. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.43.4.853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ellis, B. J., & Symons, D. (1990). Sex differences in sexual fantasy: An evolutionary psychological approach. Journal of Sex Research, 27, 527–555. doi: 10.1080/00224499009551579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fehring, R. J. (2005). New low- and high-tech calendar methods of family planning. Journal of Midwifery and Women’s Health, 50, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jmwh.2004.07.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gagnon, J. H., & Simon, W. (1973). Sexual conduct. Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
  15. Gangestad, S. W., Garver-Apgar, C. E., Simpson, J. A., & Cousins, A. J. (2007). Changes in women’s mate preferences across the ovulatory cycle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 151–163. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.151.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Christensen, P. N. (2004). Women’s preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, 15, 203–207. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preference for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 265, 927–933. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005). Women’s sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle depend on primary partner developmental instability. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 272, 2023–2027. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2010a). Men’s facial masculinity predicts changes in their female partners’ sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle, whereas men’s intelligence does not. Evolution of Human Behavior, 31, 412–424. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.06.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2010b). Fertility in the cycle predicts women’s interest in sexual opportunism. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 400–411. doi: 10.1013/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Giambra, L. (1974). Daydreaming across the life span: Late adolescent to senior citizen. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 5, 115–140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Green, A. S., Rafaeli, E., Bolger, N., Shrout, P. E., & Reis, H. T. (2006). Paper or plastic? Data equivalence in paper and electronic diaries. Psychological Methods, 11, 87–105. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.1.87.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hariton, B. E., & Singer, J. L. (1974). Women’s fantasies during sexual intercourse: Normative and theoretical implications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 313–322. doi: 10.1037/h0036669.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hunt, M. (1974). Sexual behavior in the 70’s. Chicago: Playboy Press.Google Scholar
  25. Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Nordsiek, U., Luzar, C., Young, A. W., & Banse, R. (2010). Viewing time effects revisited: Prolonged response latencies for sexually attractive targets under restricted task conditions. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 1275–1288. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9595-2.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Israel, E., & Strassberg, D. S. (2009). Viewing time as an objective measure of sexual interest in heterosexual men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 551–558. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9246-4.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Iwawaki, S., & Wilson, G. D. (1983). Sex fantasies in Japan. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 543–545. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(83)90086-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jones, J. C., & Barlow, D. H. (1990). Self reported frequency of sexual urges, fantasies, and masturbatory fantasies in heterosexual males and females. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 269–279. doi: 10.1007/BF01541552.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
  30. Knoth, R., Boyd, K., & Singer, B. (1988). Empirical tests of sexual selection theory: Predictions of sex differences in onset, intensity, and time course of sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 24, 73–89. doi: 10.1080/00224498809551399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Landolt, M. A., Lalumière, M. L., & Quinsey, V. L. (1995). Sex differences in intra-sex variations in human mating tactics: An evolutionary approach. Ethology & Sociobiology, 16, 3–23. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(94)00012-V.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Leitenberg, H., & Henning, K. (1995). Sexual fantasy. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 469–496. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.469.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Meuwissen, I. (1990). Fantasy induced sexual arousal across the menstrual cycle. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Australia.Google Scholar
  34. Miller, P. Y., & Simon, W. (1980). The development of sexuality in adolescence. In J. Abelson (Ed.), Adolescent psychology (pp. 383–407). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  35. Pacific Psychological Assessment Corporation (PPAC). (2004). The NRP (Not Real People) stimulus set for assessment of sexual interest. Victoria, BC: Author.Google Scholar
  36. Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2000). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically: Further evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00033-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Person, E. S., Terestman, N., Myers, W. A., Goldberg, E., & Salvadori, C. (1989). Gender differences in sexual behaviors and fantasies in a college population. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 15, 187–198. doi: 10.1080/00926238908403822.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Purifoy, F. E., Grodsky, A., & Giambra, L. M. (1992). The relationship of sexual daydreaming to sexual activity, sexual drive, and sexual attitudes for women across the lifespan. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 369–385. doi: 10.1007/BF01542026.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Royston, J. P., & Abrams, R. M. (1980). An objective method for detecting the shift in basal body temperature in women. Biometrics, 36, 217–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rullo, J. E., Kinnish, K., & Strassberg, D. S. (2006). Sex differences in the specificity of sexual behavior, fantasy, and romantic attraction. Poster presented at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
  41. Rullo, J. E., Strassberg, D. S., & Israel, E. (2010). Category-specificity in sexual interest in gay men and lesbians. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 874–879. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9497-3.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sierra, J. C., Ortega, V., & Zubeidat, I. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis of a Spanish version of the Sex Fantasy Questionnaire: Assessing gender differences. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 32, 137–159. doi: 10.1080/00926230500442318.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An introduction to the experimental study of inner experience. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  44. Slob, A. K., Bax, C. M., Hop, W. C. J., Rowland, D. L., & ten Bosch, J. J. (1996). Sexual arousability and the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 21, 545–558. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(95)00058-5.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Stanislaw, H., & Rice, F. J. (1988). Correlation between sexual desire and menstrual cycle characteristics. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 17, 499–508. doi: 10.1007/BF01542338.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Strassberg, D. S., & Lowe, K. (1995). Volunteer bias in sexuality research. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24, 369–382. doi: 10.1007/BF01541853.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sue, D. (1979). Erotic fantasies of college students during coitus. Journal of Sex Research, 15, 299–305. doi: 10.1080/00224497909551053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Suschinsky, K. D., & Lalumière, M. L. (2011). Prepared for anything? An investigation of female genital arousal to rape cues. Psychological Science, 22, 159–165. doi: 10.1177/0956797610394660.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Suschinsky, K. D., Lalumière, M. L., & Chivers, M. L. (2009). Sex difference in patterns of genital sexual arousal: Measurement artifacts or true phenomena? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 559–573. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9339-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at adolescence. London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  52. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). The evolutionary biology of human female sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Wallen, K., & Rupp, H. A. (2010). Women’s interest in visual sexual stimuli varies with menstrual cycle phase at first exposure and predicts later interest. Hormones and Behavior, 57, 263–268. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.12.005.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wilson, G. D., & Lang, R. J. (1981). Sex differences in sexual fantasy patterns. Personality and Individual Differences, 2, 343–346. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(81)90093-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Samantha J. Dawson
    • 1
  • Kelly D. Suschinsky
    • 1
    • 2
  • Martin L. Lalumière
    • 1
    Email author
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of LethbridgeLethbridgeCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyQueen’s UniversityKingstonCanada

Personalised recommendations