Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Incorporating Emotions Specific to the Sexual Response into Theories of Emotion Using the Indiana Sexual and Affective Word Set

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The sexual response includes an emotional component, but it is not clear whether this component is specific to sex and whether it is best explained by dimensional or discrete emotion theories. To determine whether the emotional component of the sexual response is distinct from other emotions, participants (n = 1099) rated 1450 sexual and non-sexual words according to dimensional theories of emotion (using scales of valence, arousal, and dominance) and according to theories of basic emotion (using scales of happiness, anger, sadness, fear, and disgust). In addition, ratings were provided for newly developed scales of sexual valence, arousal, and energy. A factor analysis produced four factors, together accounting for 91.5% of the variance in participant ratings. Using logistic regression analysis, we found that one word category or factor, labeled “sexual,” was predicted only by the new sexual arousal and energy scales. The remaining three factors, labeled “disgusting,” “happy,” and “basic aversive” were best predicted by basic (or discrete) emotion ratings. Dimensional ratings of valence, sexual valence, and arousal were not predictive of any of the four categories. These results suggest that the addition of sexually specific emotions to basic emotion theories is justified and needed to account fully for emotional responses to sexual stimuli. In addition, the findings provide initial validation for the Indiana Sexual and Affective Words Set (ISAWS), supporting its use in future studies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The complete supplementary database of words and ratings is available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 12, 169–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1994). Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature, 372, 669–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, M. B. (1945). Psychological differentiation of emotional states. Psychological Review, 52, 35–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnow, B. A., Desmond, J. E., Banner, L. L., Glover, G. H., Solomon, A., Polan, M. L., et al. (2002). Brain activation and sexual arousal in healthy, heterosexual males. Brain, 125, 1014–1023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999a). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual, and affective ratings. Gainesville: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999b). International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS): Stimuli, instruction manual, and affective ratings. Gainesville: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: Behavior, feeling, and physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 242–276). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., & Young, A. W. (2000). Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1077–1078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., & Young, A. W. (2001). Neuropsychology of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 2, 352–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Rowland, D., Perrett, D. I., Hodges, J. R., & Etcoff, N. L. (1996). Facial emotion recognition after bilateral amygdala damage differentially sever impairment of fear. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 699–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., Miyake, K., & Bradshaw, D. (1992). Japanese and American infants’ response to arm restraint. Developmental Psychology, 28, 578–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carretie, L., Hinojosa, J. A., & Mercado, F. (2003). Cerebral patterns of attentional habituation to emotional visual stimuli. Psychophysiology, 40, 381–388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1973). Facial expression of emotion in nonhuman primates. In P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial expression (pp. 11–90). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (2000). A second chance for emotion. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 12–23). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dickson, A., & Dearing, M. F. (1979). Appetitive-aversive interactions and inhibitory processes. In A. Dickson & R. A. Boakes (Eds.), Mechanisms of learning and motivation (pp. 203–231). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, R. J., & Morris, J. S. (2000). The functional anatomy of innate and acquired fear: Perspectives from neuroimaging. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 225–241). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, E. (1941). An explanation of “emotional” phenomena without the use of the concept “emotion”. Journal of General Psychology, 25, 283–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1973). Cross-cultural studies of facial expression. In P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial expression (pp. 169–222). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalfleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 45–60). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Everaerd, W. (1988). Commentary on sex research: Sex as an emotion. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 1(2), 3–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredericks, C. A., Kalmar, J. H., & Blumberg, H. P. (2006). The role of the ventral prefrontal cortex in mood disorders. In D. H. Zald & S. L. Rauch (Eds.), The orbitofrontal cortex (pp. 545–578). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H., & Sundararajan, L. (2007). Emotion refinement: A theory inspired by Chinese poetics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 227–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heiman, J. R. (1980). Female sexual response patterns: Interactions of physiological, affective, and contextual cues. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 1311–1316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israel, E., & Strassberg, D. S. (2009). Viewing time as an objective measure of sexual interest in heterosexual men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 551–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, E., Everaerd, W., Spiering, M., & Janssen, J. (2000). Automatic processes and the appraisal of sexual stimuli: Toward an information processing model of sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 37, 8–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, E., Prause, N., & Geer, J. H. (2007). The sexual response. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinari, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 245–266). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Karama, S., Lecours, A. R., Leroux, J. M., Bourgouin, P., Beaudoin, G., Joubert, S., et al. (2002). Areas of brain activation in males and females during viewing of erotic film excerpts. Human Brain Mapping, 16, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Processing emotional pictures and words: Effects of valence and arousal. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 6, 110–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S. W., Sohn, D. W., Cho, Y.-H., Yang, W. S., Lee, K.-U., Juh, R., et al. (2006). Brain activation by visual erotic stimuli in healthy middle aged males. International Journal of Impotence Research, 18, 452–457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain: An interdisciplinary approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koukounas, E., & McCabe, M. P. (2001). Sexual and emotional variables influencing sexual response to erotica: A psychophysiological investigation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 30, 393–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laan, E., & Everaerd, W. (1995). Determinants of female sexual arousal: Psychophysiological theory and data. Annual Review of Sex Research, 6, 32–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Gainesville: University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 684–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, D. B. (1951). Emotion. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 473–516). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). An approach to environmental psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 74–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikels, J. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Larkin, G. R., Lindberg, C. M., Maglio, S. J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2005). Emotional category data on images from the International Affective Picture System. Behavioral Research Methods, 37, 626–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation and social learning. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of science (Vol. 2, pp. 196–292). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mosher, D. L., & MacIan, P. (1994). College men and women respond to X-rated videos intended for male and female audiences: Gender and sexual scripts. Journal of Sex Research, 31, 99–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Sullivan, L. F., & Gaines, M. E. (1998). Decision-making in college students’ heterosexual dating relationships: Ambivalence about engaging in sexual activity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 347–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Lundqvist, D. (2000). Unconscious emotion: Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysiological data and neuropsychological mechanisms. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 296–327). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, K., Kang, H. K., Seo, J. J., Kim, H. J., Ryu, S. B., & Jeong, G. W. (2001). Blood-oxygenation-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging for evaluating cerebral regions of female sexual arousal response. Urology, 57, 1189–1194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, Z. D., & Janssen, E. (2007). Ambivalent affect and sexual response: The impact of co-occurring positive and negative emotions on subjective and physiological sexual responses to erotic stimuli. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 793–807.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage, 16, 331–348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redican, W. K. (1982). An evolutionary perspective on human facial displays. In P. Ekman (Ed.), Emotion in the human face (2nd ed., pp. 212–280). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowland, D. L., Cooper, S. E., & Slob, A. K. (1996). Genital and psychoaffective response to erotic stimulation in sexually functional and dysfunctional men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 194–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rupp, H. A., & Wallen, K. (2007). Sex differences in viewing sexual stimuli: An eye-tracking study in men and women. Hormones and Behavior, 51, 524–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rupp, H. A., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in response to visual sexual stimuli: A review. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 206–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rupp, H. A., & Wallen, K. (2009). Sex-specific content preferences for visual sexual stimuli. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 417–426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2005). Response latencies of pleasure and displeasure ratings: Further evidence for mixed feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 671–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneirla, T. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 1–42). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

  • Scollon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Deiner, R. (2005). An experience sampling and cross-cultural investigation of the relation between pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 27–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sprengelmeyer, R., Rausch, M., Eysel, U. T., & Przuntek, H. (1998). Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 265, 1927–1931.

  • Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Karnat, A., Lange, H., Homberg, V., et al. (1996). Loss of disgust. Perception of faces and emotions in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119, 1647–1665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stark, R., Schienle, A., Girod, C., Walter, B., Kirsch, P., Blecker, C., et al. (2005). Erotic and disgust-inducing pictures—Differences in the hemodynamic responses of the brain. Biological Psychology, 70, 19–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., & James, T. W. (2008). Affective auditory stimuli: Characterization of the International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS) by discrete emotional categories. Behavioral Research Methods, 40, 315–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., Mikels, J. A., & James, T. W. (2007). Characterization of the affective norms for English words by discrete emotional categories. Behavioral Research Methods, 39, 1020–1024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoleru, S., Gregoire, M. C., Gerard, D., Decety, J., Lafarge, E., Cinotti, L., et al. (1999). Neuroanatomical correlates of visually evoked sexual arousal in human males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28, 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Lankveld, J. J., & Smulders, F. T. (2008). The effect of visual sexual content on the event-related potential. Biological Psychology, 79, 200–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, M., Bermpohl, F., Mouras, H., Schiltz, K., Tempelmann, C., Rotte, M., et al. (2008). Distinguishing specific sexual and general emotional effects in fMRI-subcortical and cortical arousal during erotic picture viewing. Neuroimage, 40, 1482–1494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems in affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1896). Gundriss der Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Entgelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. W., Aggleton, J. P., Hellawell, D. J., Johnson, M., Broks, P., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Face processing impairments after amygdalotomy. Brain, 118, 15–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported, in part, by the IUB Faculty Research Support Program, the Indiana University GPSO Research Grant, the Howard Hughes Medical Institute Capstone Independent Research Opportunity Program, and by a Friends of the Kinsey Institute Research Grant. The authors thank Sharada Subramanian and Amy Zhang for their assistance with this project, and Kathryn Macapagal and Ross VanDerKlok for assistance with the article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ryan A. Stevenson.

Appendix

Appendix

This appendix accompanies the Indiana Sexual and Affective Word Set (ISAWS), a set of 1450 English words. Example data included are ratings on five basic emotion scales, three emotional dimension scales, and three sexual emotion scales. Basic emotions include happiness, anger, sadness, fear, and disgust (as used in Mikels et al., 2005; Stevenson & James, 2008; Stevenson et al., 2007), emotional dimensions include valence, arousal, and dominance (as used in Bradley & Lang, 1999a, 1999b; Lang et al., 2005), and sexual emotions include sexual valence, sexual arousal, and sexual energy. Ratings were collected on 1099 students from Indiana University (516 female, 583 male, M age = 20 years). Full accounts of ratings for all 1450 words on each rating scale are available upon request from the authors in document or spreadsheet format.

Rating Scales

Rating scales for the five basic emotions of happiness, anger, sadness, fear, and disgust, emotional dimensions of arousal and dominance, and sexual-emotion dimensions of sexual arousal and sexual energy were unipolar scales from 1 (not at all…) to 9 (extremely…). Rating scales for the emotional dimension of valence and sexual-emotion dimension of sexual valence were bipolar scales ranging from 1 (extremely negative or extremely sexually negative) to 9 (extremely positive or extremely sexually positive), centered around 5 (neutral or sexually neutral). In the case that a participant did not know the word, a rating of 0 was given, and the rating was not used in mean, SD, or sex-difference calculations.

Recognition Rates

Recognition rates were calculated for each word based on the number of participants that rated the word 1-9 relative to the number of participants that rated the word 0, indicating they did not know that word. All Ns reported, as well as means, standard deviations, and sex differences, are based on the number of ratings from 1 to 9. That is, a rating of 0 by a participant was not taken into consideration for any statistic except for recognition rate.

Sex Differences

Sex differences were calculated by running a simple t-test on the raw ratings from males versus those from females. T-scores for each word are presented in the tables with all participants included. Sex-difference t-scores are reported using male-female means, and as such, a positive t-score indicates a higher rating by males, and a negative t-score indicates a higher rating by females. Using the sex-specific means, SDs, and number of ratings per sex, experimenters can calculate any level of sex difference desired for a given experimental usage of the words. This allows researchers to choose words that do not contain significant sex differences at a level of the experimenter’s choosing.

Table Organization

Examples of nine tables can be found below, with all tables found in online supplemental materials in both text and Microsoft Excel formats. These nine tables contain information on each word in the ISAWS, separated by sex (female, male, and both) and separated by emotional theory (basic, dimensional, and sexual). Words are organized according to their ISAWS number, which cluster words into semantically similar groups (i.e. words 2000–2015 are all names for male genitalia).

Appendix Table 1: Table 1 includes ratings on individual words from all subjects on the basic emotion scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, and the recognition rate of the word. Means, SDs, and sex difference t-scores are provided for happiness, fear, anger, disgust, and sadness. A range of Ns has been provided (individual Ns for each basic emotion were omitted due to lack of space, but are included in the Excel sheets available in conjunction with this technical report).

Appendix Table 2: Table 2 includes ratings on individual words from only female subjects on the basic emotion scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word by females. Means, SDs, and Ns are provided for happiness, fear, anger, disgust, and sadness.

Appendix Table 3: Table 3 includes ratings on individual words from only male subjects on the basic emotion scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word by males. Means, SDs, and Ns are provided for happiness, fear, anger, disgust, and sadness.

Appendix Table 4: Table 4 includes ratings on individual words from all subjects on the emotion dimension scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word. Means, SDs, Ns, and sex difference t-scores are provided for valence, arousal, and dominance.

Appendix Table 5: Table 5 includes ratings on individual words from only female subjects on the emotion dimension scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word. Means, SDs, and Ns are provided for valence, arousal, and dominance.

Appendix Table 6: Table 6 includes ratings on individual words from only male subjects on the emotion dimension scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word. Means, SDs, and Ns are provided for valence, arousal, and dominance.

Appendix Table 7: Table 7 includes ratings on individual words from all subjects on the sexual-emotion dimension scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word. Means, SDs, Ns, and sex difference t-scores are provided for sexual valence, sexual arousal, and sexual energy.

Appendix Table 8: Table 8 includes ratings on individual words from only female subjects on the sexual-emotion dimension scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word. Means, SDs, and Ns are provided for sexual valence, sexual arousal, and sexual energy.

Appendix Table 9: Table 9 includes ratings on individual words from only male subjects on the sexual-emotion dimension scales. Columns include the word rated, the ISAWS number, word length, and the recognition rate of the word. Means, SDs, and Ns are provided for sexual valence, sexual arousal, and sexual energy.

 

Appendix Table 1 All subjects
Appendix Table 2 Female subjects
Appendix Table 3 Male subjects
Appendix Table 4 All subjects
Appendix Table 5 Female subjects
Appendix Table 6 Male subjects
Appendix Table 7 All subjects
Appendix Table 8 Female subjects
Appendix Table 9 Male subjects

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stevenson, R.A., Stevenson, L.D., Rupp, H.A. et al. Incorporating Emotions Specific to the Sexual Response into Theories of Emotion Using the Indiana Sexual and Affective Word Set. Arch Sex Behav 40, 59–78 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-010-9669-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-010-9669-1

Keywords

Navigation