Abstract
This study used some of the data from the BBC Internet study to assess the universality of sex related spatial competencies, as these are described in the terms of Silverman and Eals’ (1992) hunter-gatherer theory of human spatial sex differences. As predicted, men scored significantly higher than women on a test of three-dimensional mental rotations in all seven ethnic groups and 40 countries used. Close to prediction, women scored significantly higher than men on a test of object location memory in all seven ethnic groups and 35 of the 40 countries. The data were discussed in terms of their implications for research approaches in this area and a paradigm for future studies was proposed, based on the interaction of innate and environmental factors in the ontogenetic development of spatial sex differences.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Notes
No data were given for United Arab Emirates.
References
Alcock, J. A. (1984). Animal behavior: An evolutionary approach (3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Barrett, R. J., & Ray, O. S. (1970). Behavior in the open field, Lashley III maze, shuttle box and Sidman avoidance as a function of strain, sex, and age. Developmental Psychology, 3, 73–77.
Berry, J. W. (1966). Temme and Eskimo perceptual skills. International Journal of Psychology, 1, 207–229.
Binnie-Dawson, J. L. M., & Cheung, Y. M. (1982). The effects of different types of neonatal feminization and environmental stimulation on changes in sex associated activity/spatial learning skills. Biological Psychology, 15, 109–140.
Burg, A. (1968). Lateral visual field as related to age and sex. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52, 10–15.
Choi, J., & Silverman, I. (1996). Sexual dimorphism in spatial behaviors: Applications to route learning. Evolution & Cognition, 2, 165–171.
Choi, J., & Silverman, I. (2003). Processes underlying sex differences in route-learning strategies in children and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1153–1166.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dabbs, J. M., Chang, E. L., Strong, R. A., & Milun, R. (1998). Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 89–98.
Eals, M., & Silverman, I. (1994). The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: Proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 95–105.
Ecuyer-Dab, I., & Robert, M. (2004a). Have sex differences in spatial ability evolved from male competition for mating and female concern for survival? Cognition, 91, 221–257.
Ecuyer-Dab, I., & Robert, M. (2004b). Spatial ability and home-range size: Examining the relationship in Western men and women (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 217–231.
Galea, L. A. M., & Kimura, D. (1993). Sex differences in route-learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 53–65.
Gaulin, S. J. C., & Fitzgerald, R. W. (1986). Sex differences in spatial ability: An evolutionary hypothesis and test. The American Naturalist, 127, 74–88.
Gaulin, S. J. C., & Hoffman, H. A. (1988). Evolution and development of sex differences in spatial ability. In L. Betzig, M. B. Mulder, & P. Turke (Eds.), Human reproductive behavior: A Darwinian perspective (pp. 129–152). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gaulin, S. J. C., Silverman, I., Phillips, K., & Reiber, C. (1997). Activational hormone influences on abilities and attitudes: Implications for evolutionary theory. Evolution and Cognition, 3, 191–199.
Gur, R. C., Alsop, D., Giahn, D., Petty, R., Swanson, C. L., Maldjian, J. A., et al. (2000). An fMRI study of sex differences in regional activation to as verbal and a spatial task. Brain and Language, 74, 157–170.
Jahoda, G. (1980). Sex and ethnic differences on a spatial-perceptual task: Some hypotheses tested. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 425–431.
James, T. W., & Kimura, D. (1997). Sex differences in remembering the locations of objects in an array: Location-shift versus location-exchanges. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 155–163.
Jardin, R., & Martin, N. G. (1983). Spatial ability and throwing accuracy. Behavior Genetics, 13, 331–340.
Jordan, K., Wüstenberg, T., Heinze, H.-J., Peters, M., & Jäncke, L. (2002). Women and men exhibit different cortical activation patterns during mental rotation tasks. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2397–2408.
Joseph, R., Hess, S., & Birecree, E. (1978). Effects of hormone manipulation and exploration on sex differences in maze learning. Behavioral Biology, 24, 364–377.
Joshi, M. S., MacLean, M., & Carter, W. (1999). Children’s journey to school: Spatial skills, knowledge and perceptions of the environment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 125–139.
Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Kolakowski, D., & Malina, R. M. (1974). Spatial ability, throwing accuracy and man’s hunting heritage. Nature, 251, 410–412.
Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30, 765–779.
Lawton, C. A. (1996). Strategies for indoor wayfinding: The role of orientation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, 137–145.
Lawton, C. A., & Kallai, J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: A cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles, 47, 389–401.
Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, S. F., Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2005). Socioeconomic status modifies the sex difference in spatial skill. Psychological Science, 16, 841–845.
Lynn, R. (1992). Sex differences on the differential aptitude test in British and American adolescents. Educational Psychology, 12, 101–106.
Mann, V. A., Sasanuma, S., Sakuma, N., & Masaki, S. (1990). Sex differences in cognitive abilities: A cross-cultural perspective. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1063–1077.
McBurney, D. H., Gaulin, S. J. C., Devineni, T., & Adams, C. (1997). Superior spatial memory of women: Stronger evidence for the gathering hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 165–174.
McGivern, R. F., Huston, J. P., Byrd, D., King, T., Siegle, G. J., & Reilly, J. (1997). Sex related differences in attention in adults and children. Brain and Cognition, 34, 323–336.
Moffat, S. D., Hampson, E., & Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation in a “virtual” maze: Sex differences and correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 73–87.
Muller, R. U., Bostock, E. M., Taube, J. S., & Kubie, J. L. (1994). On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 7235–7251.
Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1971). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability in an East African society. Journal of Social Psychology, 83, 15–22.
Nerlove, S. B., Munroe, R. H., & Munroe, R. L. (1971). Effects of environmental experience on spatial ability: A replication. Journal of Social Psychology, 84, 3–10.
O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon.
Packard, M. G. (1998). Posttraining estrogen and memory modulation. Hormones and Behavior, 34, 126–139.
Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test: Different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58.
Pizzamiglio, L., Guariglia, C., & Cosentino, T. (1998). Evidence for separate allocentric and egocentric space processing in neglect patients. Cortex, 34, 719–730.
Porteus, S. D. (1965). Porteus maze test: Fifty years’ application. Palo Alto, CA: Pacific Books.
Reimers, S. (2007). The BBC Internet study: General methodology. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36. DOI 10.1007/s10508-006-9143-2.
Saucier, D. M., Green, S. M., Leason, J., MacFadden, A., Bell. S., & Elias, L. J. (2002). Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies? Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 403–410.
Schmitz, S. (1997). Gender-related strategies in environmental development: Effects of anxiety on wayfinding in and representation of a three-dimensional maze. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17, 215–228.
Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimen-sional objects. Science, 171, 701–703.
Silverman, I., & Choi, J. (2005). Locating places. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The evolutionary psychology handbook (pp. 177–199). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Silverman, I., Choi, J., MacKewn, A., Fisher, M., Moro, J., & Ols-hansky, E. (2000). Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding: Further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 201–213.
Silverman, I., & Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 531–549). New York: Oxford Press.
Silverman, I., & Phillips, K. (1998). The evolutionary psychology of spatial sex differences. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues and applications (pp. 595–611). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Silverman, I., Phillips, K., & Silverman, L. K. (1996). Homogeneity of effect sizes for sex across spatial tests and cultures: Implications for hormonal theories. Brain and Cognition, 31, 90–94.
Taube, J. S. (1995). Head direction cells recorded in the anterior thalamic nuclei in freely moving rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 70–86.
Taube, J. S. (1998). Head directional cells and the neurophysiological basis for a sense of direction. Progress in Neurobiology, 3, 225–256.
Taube, J. S., Muller, R. U., & Ranck, J. B. (1990). Head direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats: Description and qualitative analysis. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 420–435.
Total GNI 2004, Atlas method. Retrieved February 13, 2006, from the World Development Indicators database, World Bank Web site: http://web.worldbank.org/WBSITE/EXTERNAL/DATASTATISTICS/0,contentMDK:20399244∼menuPK:1192694∼pagePK:64133150∼piPK:64133175∼theSitePK:239419,00.html
Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations: A group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.
Watson, N. V., & Kimura, D. (1991). Nontrivial sex differences in throwing and intercepting: Relation to psychometrically-defined spatial functions. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 375–385.
Williams, C. L., Barnett, A. M., & Meck, W. H. (1990). Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation of spatial memory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 84–97.
Williams, C. L., & Meck, W. H. (1991). The organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 16, 155–176.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Silverman, I., Choi, J. & Peters, M. The Hunter-Gatherer Theory of Sex Differences in Spatial Abilities: Data from 40 Countries. Arch Sex Behav 36, 261–268 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9168-6
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9168-6
Keywords
- Sex differences
- Spatial abilities
- Evolutionary theory
- Hunter-gatherer
- Mental rotations
- Object location memory