Abstract
Based on a sample of nearly 8,000 college students, this study was undertaken to determine the prevalence of non-heterosexuality using four different measures of sexual orientation: a self-identity measure, an attraction measure, a fantasy measure, and an experiential measure. Over 97% of both males and females labeled themselves as heterosexual, with the proportion of homosexuals and bisexuals combined constituting nearly 3% of the male sample and about 2% of the female sample. Roughly 80–85% of both sexes would be classified as exclusively heterosexual, with about 10% of both sexes having at least half of their sexual fantasies involving same-sex partners. In terms of sexual experiences, about 5% of non-virgin males and 0.5% of non-virgin females reported that all of their experiences had involved same-sex partners. After examining each of the sexual orientation measures separately, we compared them to one another. These comparisons revealed an unsettling number of apparent contradictions in the responses given by individual participants (e.g., participants who stated that they were homosexual but that all of their fantasies involved members of the opposite sex). By eliminating participants who provided these apparent contradictory responses, a post-hoc refined self-identified measure of sexual orientation was created. This refinement had essentially no effect on our estimates of the proportion of college students who were identified as heterosexual or homosexual, thus suggesting that the inconsistencies were largely due to random response error. Boosting our confidence in the post-hoc refined measure, we found that it exhibited slightly stronger links with several established childhood correlates of sexual orientation (such as the enjoyment of collecting dolls and playing dress-up) than was true for the original self-identified sexual orientation.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Alexander, C. S., Somerfield, M. R., Ensminger, M. F., Johnson, K. E., & Kim, Y. J. (1993). Consistency of adolescents' self-report of sexual behavior in a longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 455–471.
Bagley, C., & Tremblay, P. (1998). On the prevalence of homosexuality and bisexuality, in a random community survey of 750 men aged 18 to 27. Journal of Homosexuality, 36, 1–18.
Bailey, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation: A conceptual analysis and quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 31, 43–55.
Billy, J. O., Tanfer, K., Grady, W. R., & Klepinger, D. H. (1993). The sexual behavior of men in the United States. Family Planning Perspectives, 25, 52–60.
Ellis, L. (1994). Research methods in the social sciences. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.
Ellis, L. (1996). Theories of homosexuality. In R. C. Savin-Williams & K. M. Cohen (Eds.), The lives of lesbians, gays, and bisexuals (pp. 11–32). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
Ellis, L., Burke, D., & Ames, M. A. (1987). Sexual orientation as a continuous variable: A comparison between the sexes. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 523–529.
Ellis, L., & Cole-Harding, S. (2001). The effects of prenatal stress, and of prenatal alcohol and nicotine exposure, on human sexual orientation. Physiology and Behavior, 74, 213–226.
Ellis, L., & Hellberg, J. (2004). Fetal exposure to prescription drugs and adult sexual orientation: Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 225–236.
Kirk, K. M., Bailey, J. M., & Martin, N. G. (1999). How accurate is the family history method for assessing siblings' sexual orientation? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28, 129–137.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
McConaghy, N. (1987). Heterosexuality/homosexuality: Dichotomy or continuum. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 411–424.
McConaghy, N., Armstrong, M. S., Birrell, P. C., & Buhrich, N. (1979). The incidence of bisexual feelings and opposite sex behavior in medical students. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 167, 685–688.
McFarlane, M., & St. Lawrence, J. S. (1999). Adolescents' recall of sexual behavior: Consistency of self-report and effect of variations in recall duration. Journal of Adolescent Health, 25, 199–206.
Rogers, J. L., Billy, J. O. G., & Udry, J. R. (1982). The rescission of behaviors: Inconsistent responses in adolescent survey data. Social Science Research, 11, 280–296.
Rogers, P. (1993, February 15). How many gays are there? Newsweek, p. 46.
Rogers, S. M., & Turner, C. F. (1991). Male-female sexual contact in the U.S.A.: Findings from five sample surveys, 1970–1990. Journal of Sex Research, 28, 491–519.
Upchurch, D. M., Lillard, L. A., Aneshensel, C. S., & Fang Li, N. (2002). Inconsistencies in reporting the occurrence and timing of first intercourse among adolescents. Journal of Sex Research, 39, 197–206.
Walling, D. R. (1997). Gay and lesbian issues. In D. R. Walling (Ed.), Hot buttons: Unraveling 10 controversial issues in education (pp. 147–166). Bloomington, IN: Phi Delta Kappa Educational Foundation.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Ellis, L., Robb, B. & Burke, D. Sexual Orientation in United States and Canadian College Students. Arch Sex Behav 34, 569–581 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-005-6283-8
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-005-6283-8